transcript
If You’re Reading This, You’re Probably ‘WEIRD’
The anthropologist Joseph Henrich parses how culture shapes our psyches.
[MUSIC PLAYING]
- ezra klein
So here’s the thing. If you’re listening to this podcast, you’re pretty WEIRD. You’re probably very weird, and not just for all the obvious reasons you’re thinking of. In social science, or at least certain corners of it, WEIRD is now an acronym. It stands for a certain kind of person: western, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic.
And WEIRD people, who have been the people we’ve been surveying and studying for a lot of research on psychology, they actually turn out to be different, much more so than they, than we often realize or admit. There are all these things we take for granted as basic elements of human psychology and ethics that are actually peculiar to the WEIRD psychology.
We take them for granted because we feel them. We take them for granted because we study ourselves and then use that to extrapolate to human nature, but we shouldn’t. The idea that we have a stable self that exists across all contexts, that a person’s intentions should be central to any evaluation of their actions, that guilt is a widely felt emotion, that self-esteem is crucial for happiness, we treat all these as truisms, but they’re not.
At least that’s the argument made by Joseph Henrich. Henrich is an anthropologist at Harvard who has done really deep, rich cross-cultural research in how different forms of human culture shape our psychologies, and into what those psychologies actually are. His 2015 book “The Secret of our Success” argued that what sets human beings apart from other species is our capacity for cultural learning.
His 2020 book “The Weirdest People in the World” takes that argument and extends it, arguing that beginning sometime in the Middle Ages, certain cultural and, really, religious shifts radically transformed the psychologies of individuals living in Europe. And that, then, the emergence of this WEIRD psychology was a prerequisite to everything from the development of market economies to representative government to human rights.
It’s a really fascinating argument. And if you take it seriously, it says something really quite profound about the indirect and unusual ways that human beings and human cultures evolve.
As always, my email: [email protected].
[MUSIC PLAYING]
Joe Henrich, welcome to the show.
- joseph henrich
It’s good to be with you.
- ezra klein
So the premise of your book, is that you, and me, and basically all the people listening to this podcast, and virtually all the people in the studies on which we have all based our ideas about what people are like are a little weird or a little distinctive? Tell me how.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so the main thing is that a lot of what you read in a psychology textbook or any of your typical psychology papers come from sampling one particular population. And as psychologists, and anthropologists, and economists began to measure psychology around the world, we found a great deal of variation along things like individualism, the relevance of shame versus guilt, the importance of analytic versus holistic thinking, the role of intentionality and things like moral judgment, and a number of other areas — time thrift, temporal discounting, and I could keep going. But there’s this interesting pattern of global variation in how people think about the world.
- ezra klein
I want to go through some specific pieces of this, or maybe run a very quick experiment with the audience. So I want to go through a bunch of different pieces of this, but let me start by having us all run a little exercise here. So if you say to yourself the words “I am,” what fills in the blank? Just take a second.
So, for me, I think of things like I’m a journalist. I’m a Californian. But there’s a study on this you write about, Joe. And I was wondering if you could talk through the “I Am” study.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, it’s called the 20 Statements Test. And they ask people to fill in the I am and then blank, or the who am I is another way of approaching it. And they just look at all the things that people respond. And so when you do this with populations in the U.S., say, people say things like I’m smart, I’m a kayaker, I’m curious. All these kind of things that relate to their attributes and their accomplishments and their aspirations. So things about themselves as an individual.
But when you do this in other places, people very quickly translate the question into things about their relationships. So I might say, I am a father, I am a brother, I’m a member of a certain group. And those tend to dominate it. So you get many more things about relationships as opposed to things about attributes, accomplishments, and aspirations. So this has to do with how people think about themselves. Am I a node in a relational network, or am I a unitary thing with my own unique attributions and ways of approaching the world?
- ezra klein
And how big are these differences? Because, obviously, there’s going to be some overlap in the curve here.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, at the extremes, American undergraduates will give you no relationships. They never mentioned that they’re a child or something like that, a son or something. And then if you go somewhere like the Maasai in Kenya, they’ll give you almost all relational attributes. So it runs the gamut of the spectrum. But, of course, lots of places are somewhere in the middle.
- ezra klein
What is a place that is in the middle? And not just on this, but in a bunch of the things we’ll talk about. When we think about the poles these different personality typologies can be, what is a place that seems to be more balanced between them than others?
- joseph henrich
Right. The famous psychologist Richard Nisbett started doing comparisons between immigrants from Asia that were attending the University of Michigan, and then European-descent Americans at the University of Michigan. And he was finding differences. So then people started studying Japan, and China, and Korea, places like that. And so it was thought that there was this big difference between Asia, the East, and the West.
But it turns out that for lots of things, Asia is actually somewhere in the middle. And you’ve got to go to places like the Maasai in Kenya or somewhere like that, where you have completely different social structure and whatnot to get the full range of difference.
- ezra klein
So in the book, you call at least part of this psychological profile that we’re talking about in the West the individualism complex. Tell me what that is.
- joseph henrich
Right. So for a long time, psychologists and anthropologists have been talking about this individualism complex. And I think at the core of it is the notion that we think of ourselves as a unitary selves and not as a node in a relational network. And that tends to have clustering around it — things like overconfidence, a reliance on guilt versus shame, a tendency of self-enhancement — so putting your best foot forward, emphasizing your attributes, and suppressing your deficits or deficiencies. Things like that.
- ezra klein
And you make the point that these different psychological approaches, they don’t just emerge for no reason, that there are ways of navigating and succeeding in different cultural institutions. So what is the individualism complex adaptive to? What would make it be the thing that we would select for?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, that’s a great way of putting the question. The way to think about it is, in the world that individualists are trying to adapt to, there are small families. And most of your relationships are optional and potentially ephemeral. So there’s a marketplace for finding friends, business partners, and marriage partners. The individual has a lot of choice in this. And you’re cultivating a unique self, so you’re trying to emphasize those traits which will make you interesting to possible friends, possible mates, and possible business partners. So it could be honesty, intelligence. These traits tend to be dispositional in the sense that they are operative across context. So when someone says you’re honest, they don’t mean you’re just honest with your friends and then dishonest everybody else. The suggestion is is that you have this trait that stretches across lots of different kinds of interactions.
Whereas, in other places, you’re born into a network, and you get, by virtue of your birth, lots of responsibilities, lots of social connections, definitions about who you’re likely to marry. There could be arranged marriages involved. So, really, what your job is is to figure out how to prosper within this prebuilt network. If you need to set up a business partnership, for example, you’ll look not for someone who’s trustworthy and smart, which you might do in the other world, but instead, you’re looking for someone who is connected to you through lots of social ties because those social ties will make that person trustworthy.
- ezra klein
You just touched on something that I found fascinating when I read about it, which is that when we talk about people in the WEIRD basket, there’s more of a sense of stability in our sense of self. I’m curious to hear you expand on that a bit. How stable, how unstable is this in other places, and why would stability be prized, particularly in a situation where you have more choice? You might think in a situation with more choices, and more options, and more capacity to move between different kinds of institutions and groups and people, that it would select for immutability, right? You can be this here and that there. That it doesn’t isn’t intuitive to me, so I’m curious why you think it is.
- joseph henrich
The key idea is that there could be reputational effects. And in the individualistic world, a lot of people are in the same category, sort of like strangers who you interact with who you might turn out to have a profitable interaction with, mutually beneficial. Could be a friendship, could be a business partnership. And so you want to be known as someone who’s honest or someone who’s intelligent across all these different contexts.
Whereas, in the relational world, you’ll have some kinds of cousins, for example, that you have a, quote, joking relationship. So anthropologists have documented the joking relationship between what they call cross cousins, which is when your parents are opposite sex. And that’s a very funny, playful relationship. But then with other kinds of cousins, the same genetic distance, you’ll have to have a relationship of respect. You’ll have to defer to them in conversation. There’s no joking around. The same thing you might have your father, and then your father’s brother. If it’s an older brother, you have to really defer to that older brother. And you wouldn’t speak in his presence, and you take his orders, and that kind of thing. So you just have to be mutable across these different contexts. How you’re going to behave with your professor is quite different than how you’re going to behave with your friends.
And you can see that flattening increasing probably even in our lifetime. So my undergraduates show up in my office at Harvard and say, hey, Joe, how’s it going? That gives you a sense of the just general stranger category.
- ezra klein
Why do you think we have seen that flattening? I mean, this had gotten a lot flatter in America than it’s been in other places. I take your point that it is getting flatter still. There seems to be a general trend against almost any kind of formality, right? I just moved away from San Francisco.
But something that has always struck me there is how much the fashion is flattened. The very, very rich people just shop at REI and wear a lot of Patagonia and REI-branded vests. And this goes back to Bill Gates, and Mark Zuckerberg, and hoodies. But there is something there about trying to, at least aesthetically, collapse distinctions and create a kind of flatness that would not have been even seen as desirable in other times. Whether it’s true, I think we can argue that. I don’t think it is true, but the desire to make it seem true is very present.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, and I think that’s just the spreading of this notion of egalitarian individualism. We’re all just individuals. We have these traits and we can interact freely, and we don’t have to be scared or anything of other people. Whereas in lots of societies, the very structure and organization of the society is rooted in hierarchy and respect for authority.
And that has a bunch of downstream implications. It means that it’s easier to come to decisions. Politics works a little bit differently. But you might not go into business with someone, for example, that is very hierarchical at a different place than you, for example.
- ezra klein
You make a distinction between self-esteem and other esteem. Tell me about that.
- joseph henrich
Yeah. So in an individualistic society, one of the things you’re trying to do is cultivate a set of traits that will make you stand out that other people will find appealing as well. So you might have a rule, for example, that you go to the gym every day. And if you don’t go to the gym, you might feel guilty. And that could affect your self-esteem because you’re not living up to the traits that you think.
But that doesn’t induce shame. Other people don’t judge you for that necessarily. Your brother doesn’t — people don’t look down on him because you failed to go to the gym or abide by some of your other norms.
Whereas in other societies, there’s a much more shared set of rules, and things like shame affect your close relative. So if you do something really shameful, then it actually makes your family feel shame, and it lowers their reputational standards.
So there do seem to be these big differences. So the relationship between self-esteem and happiness and stuff seems to be very important in WEIRD populations, but not so important in other places. So a lot of what we think of as good psychology, clinical psychology is actually a kind of WEIRD psychology.
- ezra klein
I was really struck by this that you write that, quote, “In the few non-WEIRD societies where it has been studied, having high self-esteem and a positive view of oneself are not strongly linked to either life satisfaction or subjective well-being.” And I want to hold on that for a minute because it gets at something that I think is really profound inside of your work, which is not just that in different cultures, people act differently, or rate different things, or have a different answer to the question of I am, but that when you do shift them along these dimensions, the way you experience the world actually might be pretty perceptually different.
I think to try to inhabit the idea that having high self-esteem, thinking highly of yourself, would not affect how you felt about you or your life is pretty strange, right? That’s actually a bigger perceptual gap than I think most of us think about day-to-day. So what do you make of that finding?
Having done a lot of this work, how do you think about the contingency or distinctiveness of your own perspective? How different do you think it would be to experience a world from within the cognitive structure of some of the societies you’ve studied, or, going back into the past, some of those that you have read about?
- joseph henrich
Yeah. I think that there’s a way we can get inside of this. There’s a lot of talk in our society about cultivating your true self, and finding your passion, and this kind of thing. And if you grow up in a world where the real emphasis and the thing that everybody was supposed to do was cultivate their family, care for their elders, and people took real pride in child rearing, and social connections, and strengthening their family over time, the things that might make you happy would be fulfilling all those culturally acquired goals as opposed to achieving some kind of personal state of being unique, and special, and having set yourself apart from others. So I just see it as having gotten different kinds of goals early on.
- ezra klein
So there’s another divergence between WEIRD and non-WEIRD populations that I think is interesting here, which is the degree to which we take into account people’s intentions when we judge their actions. Can you give me a few examples of how that might differ?
- joseph henrich
So the way we measure this is with a simple anecdote. So we have a simple story that we give people where someone is at a busy market. They have a bag with a few items in it. And in one story, they put the bag down to look at some items that are displayed on a counter, and someone else comes up and takes the bag and disappears off. And then by varying the story in different ways, we can make it clear that the person had a very similar bag, which they put down right next to it, and then they accidentally picked up the wrong bag. Or a case where it’s clearly a theft and they were trying to make off with the goods.
And then we asked people questions about how blameworthy the person is, how much they should be punished, things like that. And what we find is that we go all the way from WEIRD societies where it’s all about the intention. Really, people want to kind of forgive the guy who made a mistake. All the way down to there’s no difference. The person is out their goods one way or another.
Another way to think about this, if you accidentally burn someone’s house down or if you intentionally burn someone’s house down, the bottom line is the person has no house. So it just turns out that how important those intentions are, those mental states and — you can do it with beliefs or intentions — really varies across societies from places where it’s not important at all to places where it’s super important. It’s probably the most important single factor.
- ezra klein
This predicts a bit where we’re going to go in this conversation, but a big theme of your work is about how these differing temperaments, psychologies end up netting out in cultural and governmental and societal institutions. And so how do societies that differ dramatically on this question of intentions — how do their legal, or retributive, or judicial systems end up differing?
- joseph henrich
Yeah. And so you can see this in the history of European law, where there’s a lot of discussion and logic chopping and labels for different mental states that characters might have when doing something that could be a crime. Did they intend to do it? Did they think the thing that they did could do it? So there’s all these ways that lawyers break down the mental states of the actors. And each of these has implications for the nature and details of the crime, and the degree of culpability, and the punishment, all these kinds of things.
But lots of societies have just done the simple thing. If you go back and you look at pre-Christian law codes, if you accidentally shoot a guy with an arrow, then you have to pay a certain blood price to his family. And nobody cares if the arrow glanced off of a deer or if you were just trying to kill him. And then you see this in lots of other societies where intentionality either plays no role or a smaller role than the sort of obsession over the details of the mental states.
And just to make it clear that this is also part of religion. So if you get to Protestantism, it’s all about faith, right? You can somehow get to heaven by just having faith. Faith alone. No good works matters. Whereas the Catholics are more about the good works and the faith together, right? So Protestantism is kind of extreme on how important this is. In other religions, people don’t talk a lot about mental states. It’s really like, are you doing the rituals? Are you part of the community? Are you engaged? That kind of thing.
- ezra klein
I think there can be a tendency to look at the evolution, the selection towards these psychologies or caring about intentions, or whatever it might be, as almost teleological, right? We are evolving towards a higher moral state. The arc of the universe is bending towards a kind of moral justice.
I don’t think you see it that way. And so make the case for me on some of these. For instance, the relational dynamics, or the intentional dynamics, or taking into account of attention the ways in which our particular way of looking at this may not be some better way of doing it necessarily, just different, right? When you talk to people in these societies and you try to explain the way you see it or the way the place you come from sees it, what do they tell you? What do you hear that you find convincing?
- joseph henrich
So one of the things that always sticks in my mind, I was talking — I do work in these remote islands in Fiji. And I was discussing with this old Fijian elder why it is when they have a council meeting to make decisions about how things are going to happen in the village, that only a small number of elders has a discussion with the chief. Everybody else just sits quietly in the background. And then that small committee of old guys will make a decision without any input from anybody else in the room.
And I said, why not do majority vote? Everyone could just vote, and you can make a decision that way. Naturally, I thought of that because of where I come from. And he was like, forget about women, right? We only got as far as discussing what the men might do. He says, well, how can an 18-year-old who has no children and has never taken care of the clan or the family have the same weigh-in on this decision as I have, someone who has grandchildren and who has taken care of this big, very successful clan? Those things just don’t seem like they should be the same, right? So there’s a certain logic to that. That kind of makes sense.
- ezra klein
Tell me about the passengers dilemma experiment.
- joseph henrich
So this is a kind of vignette-type experiment from sociology. And in the experiment, you’re in a car, and your friend is there. Now, you can vary — you can make it a family member, too. But in the basic version, it’s a friend. The friend is driving too fast for the conditions, a little bit recklessly, and they hit someone and kill them. And then a law case is going to occur. And your friend’s lawyer says, if you testify that your friend was going under the speed limit that he’ll get off, it’ll be fine, nobody else was there. Or you can tell the truth in court. And I’ve even presented this in class. And my classes are pretty diverse.
Some members of the class can’t believe that anybody would tell the truth in court. But this varies tremendously across populations. With Canadians, 90 percent of Canadians will tell the truth in court. At least that’s what they say they would do. Whereas in other places, it would be crazy to tell the truth in the court. You’ve got to help your friend. Your friend needs you. Aren’t you a good friend? And so those are two virtues that are trading off. But how you trade those virtues off has a big effect on the effectiveness of legal institutions, right?
- ezra klein
This is a place to go back to the question I asked a second ago, where I think the non-WEIRD response actually makes a lot of intuitive sense. In this particular case, you’re trading off the interest in a way of this faceless, impersonal institution — the legal system — over someone you care about. It’s not a case where they did something where — to go back to the point of intention very deeply — they weren’t trying to do this maliciously, right? They made a mistake anybody can make.
And so you can kind of, I think, see not just the rationality, but, in some very deep way, the morality of siding with those who are nearest to you, right? Siding with the people you care about. How, as you understand it, does that evolution away from that happen? Because I do think that would have been the much more normal reaction almost all through history. And then, eventually, you get to get to Canada. How do you get to Canada?
- joseph henrich
Well, one of the things that I try to push in the book, or try to really press on, is this idea of an interpersonal pro-sociality or morality versus an interpersonal. So one is this reality you have with strangers towards arbitrary rules, whether it’s paying your taxes, giving blood to strangers. All these things that help make society run. But it’s really kind of faceless, and you’re not really helping anybody you know. Versus the kind of more normal human morality where I got friends and family, and I want to do stuff to help them, and those are my priority.
Now, of course, everybody has that interpersonal morality. But it’s a question of how much emphasis you’re putting on one and how that you’re going to make the trade offs versus that general principle, don’t lie in court that’s required to make the system run. People can’t be always lying in court. So it’s that kind of trade-off. And then much of my book is about trying to lay out how that unfolded over centuries to get us to the place where we are today. And you shouldn’t think of today as an endpoint. I think things are dynamic, and directions are changing, and that sort of thing.
- ezra klein
Let’s talk a bit about that space of evolution, though. I’ll keep going through some of the dimensions of WEIRDness. But I am interested here because something your book pains to argue is we have a bit of — I think it’s fair to call it now a myth in WEIRD societies that what caused all this was our devotion to higher order, enlightened principles, impersonal rules. We care about markets and codes of ethics and ethical philosophy.
And I think you show pretty conclusively — and we’ll get into the way family structures changed. But I think you show pretty conclusively that religion has been just this unbelievably powerful driver of cultural evolution and, arguably, is behind a lot of the personality changes that made things the enlightenment, like believing in something like the Bill of Rights, possible. So tell me a bit about the role of religion and what you might call non-rational principles in getting us to this place where impersonal structures and principles can be so dominant.
- joseph henrich
So the first is kind of an anthropological observation that if we look at the database of religions and different societies, many societies have relatively weak, whimsical — they might have a morality like a person, but it looks quite different than the big, moralizing gods that we would find in Islam or Christianity and religions like that. So I’ve long been interested in how the beliefs in these more powerful, more moralizing gods emerged.
And myself and lots of collaborators have made the case that societies that had beliefs in these more powerful, moralizing gods were able to galvanize more cooperation in larger groups. So if you believe that God was incentivizing you with, say, heaven and hell, then that can lead people to behave in somewhat more pro-social ways and allow trade, mutually beneficial transactions, larger cooperation, cooperation and warfare.
And so simple experiments that we’ve done is — one is just to go around the world to different societies. And we’re going to remote villages in Africa and Fiji and New Guinea and stuff and just asking people questions, getting measurements of their degree to which they believe in a god that is moralizing and punishing that has control over the afterlife.
And we find that if we give people a choice between allocating money in a way that the experimenter can’t be sure what they did to their community or themselves versus some coreligionist distant stranger who they don’t know, they’re more fair. They’re not totally fair, but they’re more fair towards the stranger in this monetary allocation when they report believing in these more powerful, moralizing gods.
And then as a kind of addendum to that, you can take people who do believe in the moralizing gods and unconsciously slip them cues of religion or of their god and then have them do these monetary allocations that economists like. And then they’re more generous, or they’re more adhering to the pro-social norms. More equal divisions with the distant stranger when we’ve unconsciously slipped them cues. There’s also cool natural experiments where you can use the call to prayer. And the call of prayer gives them a little boost. They’re a little more prosocial after the call to prayer.
[MUSIC PLAYING]
- ezra klein
Would it be fair — to gloss your view of human beings here — to say that you believe much less, I think, in a human nature than is, I think, the culturally dominant story and what you tend to believe in is much more of a cultural nature, that we are actually quite plastic under different cultural equilibriums? And as such, can be moved, and changed, and shifted, and formed into many more shapes as human beings depending on what culture we grow up in then I think a lot of us appreciate now.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, I think that is a fair gloss. The only place where I’d want to shape that a bit would be I teach human nature as a general education requirement at Harvard. And the thing I emphasize in that class is that we’re a cultural species, that our reliance on this cultural learning is actually part of our evolved phenotype. So we’ve genetically evolved to come into the world as babies and start imitating and acquiring and using cues to figure out who to pay attention to, and drinking in all this.
So we have a lot of extra brain. And we have changes in our developmental cycle which allow us to acquire social norms during middle childhood, say, ages 5 to 10. We internalize them. So we use them to help us navigate the world. We’re willing to pay costs if the norm demands those costs. But that can lead to all kinds of different ways of thinking about the world because even something like whether we see a visual illusion depends on the world we construct.
So one of my favorite experiments are these the Müller-Lyer illusion, which is the two arrows where one of the arrows are in and the other of the arrows are out. Well, if you grow up in a world without carpenter corners — and anthropologists have done the work in the 1960s — you actually don’t see that illusion. So you literally see the world differently. And I think that captures a lot of some of the stuff we were talking about earlier with the role of self-esteem and stuff. You literally see the world differently.
- ezra klein
You have a great line here where you write, you can’t separate culture from psychology or psychology from biology because culture physically rewires our brains, and thereby shapes how we think. Your example there with the carpenters corners is, I think, a pretty good example of that. So how does culture rewire our brains?
- joseph henrich
The idea is that we come in with a degree of plasticity in order to help us acquire the information processing that allows us to navigate the institutional landscapes, the incentives that are built up in the institutions. So just my favorite example and the one I start the book off with is when you learn to read, you get specialized neural circuitry in your left ventral hemisphere. It impinges on some of your facial processing.
So literate people are right-biased in their facial processing, but nonliterate people are much more symmetrical in their facial processing. So what neuroscientists thought was a product of humans, a right bias in the brain in terms of processing faces, turns out to be a product of literacy because you spend all this time as a child building this machine that can read. It reads automatically. One of the fun things I say in the book is that if I show you a word, you can’t stop yourself from reading it.
Even though this is just a learned skill, it’s a thing that most cultures didn’t do over human history, but now it’s an automatic piece of our brain. It takes up neurogeography. It affects things that have nothing to do with reading, so you process speech differently once you’ve learned to read. We have a thicker corpus callosum. So we know the actual biology is different. But this is all due to this cultural value on reading.
- ezra klein
So let me then use this as the bridge over to this other set of ideas. I kind of think of you as known for two big ideas. One is WEIRD cultures, the WEIRD personality type. And, obviously, there are many other co-authors on that and everything. But the other, and in some ways a part of your work, that has shaped my thinking more is around cultural evolution.
So people are very familiar with individual evolution. The idea that there is a selection pressure and so much in who we can be traced back to what gave us biological fitness to reproduce. What is cultural evolution? How do cultures evolve? What are the pressures on them that help them do that? And then, how does that then differ from individual evolution?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so I come at this by thinking about how natural selection would have shaped our genes to give us minds that allowed us to acquire information from other people. So it begins with the individual and how they can adaptively learn, when they should learn from the group, say, some kind of conformist effect, when they should attend to prestigious people, how they should integrate information from different people.
But if everyone’s doing that over generations, you can get products. And those products can be technologies that are so complex that no individual could figure out them in their lifetime. And, eventually, people don’t even know how it works. They just use it, and it does work.
Another thing would be an institution. So institutions long predate written laws. So something like marriage that’s just a cluster of social norms about who can marry who, what the relative responsibilities are in the marriage, who pays who, that kind of thing. But then the social structure, everybody who comes after that has to grow up in this world where the social networks have been organized by some norms about who marries who and where people have to live after they get married.
So in that sense, it creates this cultural niche that people’s minds have to adapt to. And I make the case that this has been going on for over a million years and has shaped much of our genetic evolution. So our genes have had to respond to the fact that we create these cultural products.
The simplest example is fire and cooking. So there’s good evidence that that goes back pretty far. But if you look at our physiology — our stomachs and our large intestines, our teeth and stuff — we’re an animal that’s dependent on cooked food. Cooking DNA, which is the proteins. It breaks things down. Essentially, its digestion outside the body. And so it means there’s much less need for all the digestive tissue that we see in other apes. So if you think about other apes, they have this big spreading rib cage that comes out, and they have these large stomachs. That’s actually for their large intestines to give them extra space to break down fibers, and process meat, and things like that. But humans don’t have that because we cook the stuff, and then we do the digestion outside. So it’s shaped our physiology, but it’s strictly — none of us know how to make fire innately. This has to be learned from growing up in a particular place. So it’s a simple example of gene culture co-evolution. But I think that social norms, and institutions, and the creation of languages has also fed back and shaped aspects of our genetic evolution.
- ezra klein
Walk me through how human children develop compared to newborns in other species.
- joseph henrich
The first thing is that — biological anthropologists study this. And it looks like children are born premature in the sense that very young human babies are helpless and they seem to come out too early. And the main reason many think that they come out too early is that their heads are getting too big. So we have a basic primate body design. And if that head gets too big, it can’t get out through the uterus and whatnot.
And so babies come out before that head gets too big. And then the baby head continues expanding and it can be taken care of. And humans have all these extra parents, these alloparents that help. And food sharing, all that kind of stuff creates this environment where you can raise a very altricial, helpless baby in this kind of world.
But humans also have this unique period after about age five or something called middle childhood. And that’s where we have quite big brains, but we still have little bodies. And this seems to be the period when kids learn all of this stuff while growing up. So it looks like selection has monkeyed around with gestation. And then we have very short nursing periods, or shorter nursing periods than other animals, probably because we can break down the food with cooking and process in other ways.
And then we have this new period, middle childhood, where we learn all the norms and get a lot of the details of the culture. And then, finally, when we have that, then we get the body expansion. And then adolescence you can think of as apprenticeship time. So adolescence, you start doing adult-like things, working with adults, eventually getting up into proficiency by the time you’re in your 20s.
Anthropologists studying hunter-gatherers, you don’t really even get the best hunters — this is thinking about the male occupation usually — until, say, mid-30s, 40s or whatnot. So there’s a lot of knowledge that you have to learn in order to be the most competent person in the community.
- ezra klein
So we have these genes. We have these aptitudes. We have these capabilities. But we don’t — I read a lot of sci-fi that has some kind of mechanistic way of moving one culture’s ideas into the next generation, right? You stick a computer chip or a download into an A.I., or read a book about spiders where they sort of somehow encode it in a sack. But we don’t have that.
And so the societies that have out-competed each other and that sort of have won this cultural evolution game, at least for now or for a period, have developed what are fundamentally cultural technologies that have helped them cooperate and pass knowledge around. And a lot of these in your book seem to be encoded in religion. And you have this view, I think, of religion as having, at its explicit level, a story about god and/or gods, and how the world works, and moralities.
And then beneath that, a set of things like ritual, and collective dance, and other things that are really incredibly powerful technologies of cultural transmission. So can you tell me about how those two things interact? Why are the structures of religion such an important part of your book and an important part of this cultural evolution story?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so it really comes from — my background, Ph.D., is in anthropology. And when you look across diverse human societies, you don’t see the need partitioning that we tend to think about the world. A typical partitioning where we have the political scientists study politics, the economists study economics. There’s some theologians and stuff who take care of religion. But in other societies, the religion is woven throughout the production system. And then there’s rituals associated with production, which is part of exchange.
All of this is justified by mystical beliefs and reified in rituals, which people do. And we now know that rituals actually have a big effect on bonding people. And whether it’s the marching together or moving together in synchrony, or listening to music together, or just doing it repeatedly seems to have these interesting psychological effects. So societies have evolved and figured out ways to do that.
And, as you mentioned, one of the features of cultural evolution that is probably not an important feature of genetic evolution for humans is this competition among societies. So you get this sorting of different institutions. And so you get production systems and distribution systems, but they’re interwoven with religion. And religion makes them do better.
So one of the societies that I discuss in the book is the Arapesh from New Guinea. And there, if things are going poorly for the village, they’ll re-initiate some rituals. But they think they’re doing it to appease the god — their god — who they think is causing bad stuff to happen because he’s not happy. Now this often leads to good things happening because they’ve done the ritual and they’ve rebonded, and now they’re cooperating again.
So they got kind of the wrong theory about — at least in my view — they have the wrong theory about the world, but it causes them to do something that’s really helpful, which is kind of redo the rituals, rebond, and begin cooperating again as a group. So this process has affected the evolution of religions, and eventually gives us the big, powerful moralizing gods that we’re all familiar with.
- ezra klein
I’ve heard you talk about a study about which kinds of communes — and I happen to be very interested in communes — survived and thrived, or at least lasted for longer periods of time in the 20th century, and how it maps on to this question of religion as a bonding force. Can you talk about that?
- joseph henrich
So in the United States, there was a great awakening and a proliferation of communes. Some of the communes were religious and some weren’t. And then you can use this as a laboratory and then ask the question, which one lasts the longest and has the most number of members?
Well, one group that clearly won was the Mormons. And then there’s also a group called the Hutterites. Other groups like the Shakers had a norm where you weren’t allowed to have sex, right? The group could only grow by in-migration. That one didn’t make it. And, notably, the ones that were religious in context were the ones that had the longest duration. So the ones that were just a bunch of political ideologues getting together and forming a group, they tended to sputter out, and they didn’t win the long race.
- ezra klein
I took your book as being fundamentally pretty pessimistic on two things that I think are fairly widely believed right now. One is the ability to change cultures dramatically just by telling them to change. But almost every major story of change I could think of from the book was pretty indirect. You created a lot of cooperation, usually around a religious story. And that ended up having these very profound downstream impacts. Whereas the much more straightforward, we’re all going to do this because it’s the right thing to do and we’re a bunch of rationalists who’ve decided to live together in this way, it doesn’t seem to have a lot of staying power.
And so downstream from that, I think it would be intuitive for a lot of secular WEIRD people to believe they’re probably much better at shaping societies more profoundly and potently because we’re free of all this superstition and Kant and WEIRDness. But, in fact, it seems to me a lesson potentially in the book is it’s actually going to be much harder for societies to evolve if they don’t have the cooperative power provided by belief and rituals that emerge from belief in something higher, that there seems to be some kind of capacity of that to get people to do the somewhat illogical, very difficult things, like, say, don’t eat pork or take the Sabbath off, to talk about Jewish examples, but you can pick them from whatever religion you want, that end up bonding a community together at the level that allows them to achieve really great things.
Is that your view that a lot of this is indirect and that we will lose something pretty profound if we continue to secularize in terms of our ability to have this kind of cultural evolution?
- joseph henrich
I definitely agree with the first part. This kind of enlightenment notion that we built the world through rationality. And if we could just convince people to be rational, they will. So that I’m definitely pessimistic on. And I think there’s tons of examples from economic development to all kinds of things that support that.
One of the things that I’m interested in is whether we can harvest insights from religion, use of ritual, things like that to improve secular impersonal institutions, build better organizations, that kind of thing. So I’m not in favor of spreading supernatural agents because we think it’ll be effective, but I think there’s wisdom embedded in religious traditions that maybe is underused in impersonal secular institutions.
- ezra klein
I want to press on this for a minute because I don’t quite buy what you just said. I don’t 100 percent believe that you buy it because so many of the examples you give and of the actual research you give has to do with the way really truly complex rituals — I mean, very complex dances, and fasting, and collective movement — and things you just really can’t get people to do. You can’t go to people and say, you should fast once a week, and do a lot of dances, and do a lot of praying and so on, because, ultimately, this maps onto some kind of human synchronicity mechanism that’s going to help you cooperate.
The real lesson I took from the book is that religion, because it gives you a reason to do things that don’t themselves have an obvious logical justification, allows you to band together these cooperation mechanisms that kind of nothing else does.
You can’t be just in a political system — can you imagine Joe Biden coming out to the Democratic Party tomorrow and saying, listen, a really important thing for us Democrats is to work together. And as such, I am asking all of you to stop eating fish three days a week. And once every six months, I would like you all to go to the ocean, and I would like you to grab a bunch of paper and throw it in as a way of expurging your sins. He’s not going to do that because nobody would do it. But it would probably be very good for cooperation among Democrats if they did it. And this just seems to me to be an actually pretty big thing.
I think it’s often talked about, why do Mormons do so much better in America than most other groups? And they have a lot of very complex religious rituals that have sustained in ways that most other religious groups have not been able to sustain the rituals. And as such, there’s a really high level of pro-sociality and cooperation in Utah, much higher than in other places in the country.
And so it does seem to me that something is being lost that probably cannot just be gained through rationalism because it is, in some ways, the illogic and complexity of the thing itself that is working, but nobody’s going to do that precisely because it is so illogical and complex, unless they believe there is some kind of punishment that is indirect for not doing it.
- joseph henrich
The kind of thing I had in mind would be in the service of some notion of — let’s take the U.S. since that’s where we are. It would be in some notion of some kind of set of American commitments, principles — we have lots of principles, lots of commitments — which would be like national service. So you graduate high school. Everybody does either military or national service, or something like that. It’s a shared sacrifice. You’re together with your fellow Americans. You’re working. Another thing would be the ritual of saying the Pledge of Allegiance at the beginning of the day. If you want to build national identity, you need tools like that. Now, of course, I would immediately caution that if you build too strong an American identity, we kind of need a global identity at the moment. So you have to think about those kinds of things. But there are ways to introduce costly activities. And they would be justified by commitment to truth, justice, and the American way, say, or some large or some supra-U.S. commitment, or something like that. But those kinds of policies at least are ideas.
[MUSIC PLAYING]
- ezra klein
I want to loop back, then, to weirdness because one of the stories that the book is telling is that weirdness, this sort of psychological profile and the world built on top of this psychological profile, which is our governments, and our institutions, and our markets, and so much of it, you really describe this as a world the Catholic Church built. Tell me how the Catholic church built it.
- joseph henrich
I think that the oldest and most important of human institutions is the family. And WEIRD people tend to have a peculiar view of the family because many of us grew up in these monogamous nuclear households that are either loosely or not very connected to other households in the area. But that’s not the story for most of humanity, and, in fact, the kinship unit was the center of production and distribution, and often a religious center with ancestor cults and things like that. Chinese is the classic case.
And what the church unknowingly did, unwittingly did was it adopted a series of prohibitions and prescriptions that dismantled the intensive kinship systems of Europe, and broke them down over centuries into monogamous nuclear families. Now some people will be familiar with the famous incest taboos where, at first, the Catholic church around the sixth century outlaws marriage to first cousins. And then it becomes second cousins. And then it eventually stretches out the sixth cousins, which are so distant that most people — we can’t even track who your sixth cousins are.
So, essentially, a taboo is anybody in your relational circle. It includes affinal relatives, so in-laws, and it includes spiritual relatives. And it forces you to get married to people quite socially distanced from you. And so the idea is this would have begun to rewire the social network. There were also laws about inheritance, where the church wanted the individual to inherit and not the group. So it broke down collective inheritance, which is very common across lots of societies.
And this occurred over centuries. And it actually transformed the church from within, and then the church began developing secular procedures and new law procedures. And these became models, which were then used in secular systems, charter towns. Developed a network of Bishop Ricks, which is one of the ways we’re able to figure this out. We look at how long different regions of Europe were under a particular bishop, and then we look at a lot of things, but one is the contemporary psychology.
- ezra klein
One of the things that emerges from this is a set of more impersonal institutions that you really understand as helping to explain why some regions have developed in the way they have while others haven’t. I thought in many ways the strongest bit of data here is from Italy. Do you mind talking through that?
- joseph henrich
So Italy has long been a puzzle for social scientists because, on the one hand, Italy in the North is famous for the Renaissance and the emergence of the banking industry, and the South is famous for corruption, and the mafia, and whatnot. So what’s going on in Italy?
And what most people don’t realize is that northern Italy was under something called the Carolingian empire. So this is Charlemagne around the year 800. He has this large empire. He’s teamed up with the Pope, and they’re gradually creating the parish system. And one of the things that the Carolingian emperors are concerned about was implementing this marriage and the family program.
So northern Italy gets this high dosage of the marriage and family program, while Southern Italy, some of it’s under the Orthodox Church centered and Byzantine. Islamic rulers are present in places like Sicily. Sardinia seems to be outside of most of this. And so Southern Italy only gets incorporated under the Pope in a sense in part of this whole enterprise much later in history than Northern Italy is.
So what we were able to do is take, say, rates of cousin marriage from 20th century Italy, and we can use that to explain variation and corruption, whether people put money in banks, their kind of general trust, a number of different measures. And so it helps, I think, explain this puzzle that’s Italy.
- ezra klein
So I find all the evidence you present on this so great, actually. I love arguments like this. I love arguments that something we didn’t understand had effects we would have never predicted that led to the world that we now live in. And I also want to maintain some skepticism because in anything like this, we’re sort of working backwards. We can see one part of Italy one way, another one the other way. And now we’re trying to test against data we have.
And there is just a lot of correlational data here. You’re looking at, say, modern day Democratic attitudes or working hours, and then the percentage of Catholic missions in an area 500 years ago. And so I guess one question I have for you is, what is your actual confidence level that this explains a very high level of modern development? And maybe one way of asking it is that, in the past however many decades, we’ve seen a tremendous rise in East Asian countries, right? They have had developmental stories that are sort of unlike any other region in that period.
And would that have been something this would have predicted? When you kind of think of pushing the story forward as opposed to backwards, if you were sitting there in 1950 trying to imagine which countries would escape poverty and then which would be able to escape the middle income trap, would you have said, oh, yeah, South Korea, and Japan, and a couple of these are really going to do it? What is, I guess, your level of confidence that this can explain as much as you’re trying to explain with it?
- joseph henrich
Great question. So we talked a bit about Italy. I could tell you the story about Europe, which will be similar. The rest of Europe. So I think one of the ways to get at these ideas — because if you’re a historian, you could possibly think of all kinds of different contingencies. You could try to think of things that are uncontrolled for that may be relevant.
So what I like to do is let’s test it somewhere else. So there’s data from just the Democratic Republic of Congo. So totally different place. Don’t tell me any stories about particular European rulers or anything particular to Europe. What we have is the city of Kananga where people migrate from villages all around the DRC. And those villages are varying distances from historical Catholic missions.
So if we look at those people today, those from villages that were closer to historical Catholic missions have simpler families, fewer kinship ties, and a more moral universalistic psychology. So they have more of the WEIRD psychology that I think traces to this taking a part of the family.
OK, let’s go to China. We can test it in China because now the church is out of the picture, but the details of the agricultural systems affect the intensity of these kinship systems. So in rice growing parts of China, you get very intensive clans. We can check that with data on clan genealogies going back 1,000 years.
And then we can look and see if that can explain contemporary variation among Han Chinese. So throw out anybody who’s not Han. Just look at Han Chinese. So you get rid of any messy variation. And then we find that people from counties or provinces that have differing amounts of rice growing have the expected changes in psychology.
We can do a similar thing in India. And then, most recently, some colleagues from the University of British Columbia looked at the United States. And so cousin marriage began to pop up in parts of the U.S., and then secular laws outlawed cousin marriage. And so you can see a decline in those subgroups, in those kinds of families that were intermarrying, and then long-term economic prosperity — so higher incomes. So it’s kind of a natural experiment because different states in the U.S. cranked this on at different times. And because we have the continuous U.S census, or at least my colleagues at the University of British Columbia do, they can show these long-term effects.
So there’s just a lot of different converging lines of evidence that help build my confidence that there might be something to this. And then, of course, as a scientist, I’m like, well, what’s the alternative explanation you want to offer for this? And then I want to let those compete.
- ezra klein
And I actually believe there’s definitely something to it. But I wonder how much when we think about the pace of global development, and the sort of weight of what is needed to achieve these institutions and the sort of impersonal nature of them, and the ability to have a society where you’re not operating in high-trust kinship networks that allows for very modern forms of economic cooperation.
And, I guess, one thing that makes me wonder about the weight given to Catholicism here is that it would make me think that development in much of Latin America and Africa would have been stronger than it’s been, particularly around these kinds of institutions and their level of corruption and so on.
Because if you just run a search on what countries have very, very high levels of penetration of the Catholic church, you’ll get a lot of the countries in Latin America and in Africa, but you wouldn’t see that line up all that cleanly with the kinds of markets and institutions that you’re talking about spring from this.
So how do you think about the places where there is a lot of Catholicism, but you don’t seem to be seeing the effects that you’re claiming for it?
- joseph henrich
I want to finish the other half of your question. So 1950 and looking at East Asia, what you see there is the adoption of a lot of Western civil law. So China ends polygyny. It burns the genealogies. It’s trying to destroy its clans. The one child policy, which comes later, that ends all cousins. So if you don’t have cousins, you can’t have cousin marriage.
So they essentially do in a period of about five decades what the church took 500 years to do in terms of completely altering the family structure. So they’re implementing the marriage and family program that the church implemented, and they have the full power of the Chinese state to do it.
And the Japanese actually get started on this first. In 1880, the Meiji Restoration is about actually copying in some cases literal civil codes that were copied from Europe that have all these rules about marriage and the family. And in societies with high levels of conformity, hierarchical decision making, you can implement these things much more quickly than amongst a bunch of disagreeable individualists.
So what would be great is if I could predict the future, right? But, at least based on that, the kind of East Asian part fits. Now we’ve tried to deal with your other question in the following way. We have nighttime satellite illumination data from the entire world. And we think we can use that as a proxy for economic growth. And then we have the map which appears in the book of the intensity of different kinship structures around the world.
And what we can do is we can look at places like Latin America and Africa and elsewhere, and we just compare different ethnic groups in the same country. So we’re removing all the country level stuff, country level politics from this and just comparing ethnic groups within the same country. And what we find is that ethnic groups that had more intensive kinship traditionally are darker, averaging data from right around 2010 or so. So it looks like economic prosperity measured as light illumination is associated with groups that traditionally had weaker kinship intensity.
And our story is that because the institutions built in Europe are the ones that have expanded around the world and now dominate, they fit within the kind of psychology and the social structure that goes along with low-intensity kinship structure. And so it’s easier for them to engage in world markets and become more prosperous.
- ezra klein
One thing I find interesting about the point you just made on some of the East Asian societies is that it does go in the opposite direction of one of the concerns I was raising earlier, which is what you’re saying is that they were able to impose, much more directly and much more by force, what the Catholic Church did indirectly, right? If the Catholic Church were saying, well, God doesn’t want you to do this, China just said, well, we don’t want you to do this. And, actually, we’re going to put a much sharper constraint around it than anyone else will.
And I’m not here to support the one child policy. I do not support the one child policy, but it does, I think, suggest — and not, again, taking the point that China went way, way, way too far on this and had very bad years as part of that, it does in a way suggest that there is the ability to run this kind of social cooperation and limiting game more directly.
- joseph henrich
Yeah. Although, the interesting part — I guess, China is kind of a test case moving forward because it’s easiest for China to do that earlier on the more hierarchical, the more conformist people are. As that process begins to move along and people are becoming more individualistic and whatnot, the ability of the state to do that, to work through elders, to rely on conformity and stuff, will get trickier. Although, I guess technology could compensate.
- ezra klein
I actually had wanted to ask you about technology because you’ve talked a lot here about the ways in which cultural evolution can be set off and can run indirectly through religion and other kinds of things that create a lot of human cooperation. But technology also seems to me to be a force that certain technologies will emerge. The birth control pill is a very canonical example here, but you can also think about the factory or air conditioning.
And all kinds of new forms of cooperation, new settlement patterns, new family structures, new ways of structuring out the life cycle begin to emerge from that. So how do you think about technology and technological change within the context of cultural evolution?
- joseph henrich
The first point is I agree that for sure technology can have those kinds of effects. I think about technology as itself a product of cultural evolution. The way social science kind of works, sometimes, people want to put technology in its own bin separate from culture, but it’s very clear that people learn. If you just go to the smallest scale of human societies, people learn how to make the relevant tools from other members of their community. This is passed on.
If you look at the larger scale societies, people still have to learn how to do whatever engineering stuff in the factories, making the tools from other people. This could be passed along indirectly through books and things like that. But there’s lots of tacit knowledge that goes into even modern industrial production that has to be moved from one craftsperson or one engineer to the next. So it’s all cultural transmission all the way down.
And one of the things I focus on the book is, what are the conditions that lead to more rapid technological change? So I have this idea of the collective brain, which is that the particular institutions in a certain place and the psychology, trust in strangers, things like that, lack of conformity will actually energize and create more rapid innovation.
So at the end of “The Weirdest People in the World,” I’m trying to explain the Industrial Revolution, and why did you have this explosion of new technologies, which did change things in the way you mentioned, all at once beginning in the English Midlands and then spreading out from there?
- ezra klein
And talk me through a little bit more your explanation for that.
- joseph henrich
Yeah. So my story is that most ideas are recombinations of existing ideas already circulating in the cultural milieu. So the more diverse minds are able to interact and share ideas, the more likely you are to create novel recombinations and lead to the diffusion of ideas. Even the famous geniuses like Einstein, if you look at special relativity, it’s a combination of a set of ideas that others were in the process of putting together, and he just happened to be the first guy to really congeal them together.
But this is true of lots of different technologies. In a book I’m writing now, we actually have data from the complete U.S Patent Database showing that nearly all patents are actually combinations of existing technologies. So that seems to be the case. And then larger cities, as a piece of evidence, tend to produce more patents per capita. So more creativity per capita. And that’s just the idea that there are more people closer together, and it kind of makes everyone smarter. You’re more likely to bump into someone who has something useful that you put together with something else, and then you’re off and running.
Things like the spread of Starbucks seems to energize patenting, which is a surprising thing to a lot of people because people had a third place where people from different companies could come together, swap ideas. And you can actually look at the details of what the new patents are and see that happening.
- ezra klein
One thing that recurs throughout some of the stories of the book is that a lot of these religious and cultural changes made possible more migration. And more migration made possible more mixing of ideas, more innovation, a lot of what we take for granted now in modernity. How do you think about migration in the modern world? Is that something that if you are kind of architecting societies for more innovation and more cultural and more rapid cultural evolution that you would be trying to turn up the dial on?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, and I have a big project on this. So we’ve combined the full complete U.S census with the complete Patent Database. And what we’re able to show is that migrant shocks to the U.S., we can look at what counties they end up in. And then we can measure the diversity of those counties by looking at last names as they appear in the census. And what we find is that what we’re calling sociocultural diversity, which is this measure of last name variation, is a huge predictor of patents in the coming decade.
And immigrant shocks add richness to that diversity. So it’s just more evidence that immigrants have really driven a lot of innovation in the U.S. And we wondered. We said, well, maybe immigrants, if you have too many, it will cause problems. It might cause problems, but we can’t find any kink in the curve. This seems to be more immigrants, more innovation.
So we looked for a hum shape. You might say, maybe there’s an optimal number of immigrants, but it doesn’t look like the U.S. has hit that. And there’s very interesting papers done by Petra Moser and colleagues showing that when the U.S. put in immigration quotas in 1924 cutting out or limiting from certain populations, you got a decline in the group and the patenting domains that those groups had previously been attributing to. And it even meant that Americans got less creative in those domains. So the immigrants are not just bringing in ideas that make themselves rich; they’re bringing in ideas that they’re mixing with the native-borns and making everybody more creative.
- ezra klein
Speaking of county-by-county data, I know something you’ve been working on is mapping, within America, how psychologically WEIRD, again, on this one scale a county is against how it seems to be voting. And I know that research isn’t out yet, but is there anything preliminarily you are seeing in the data that’s interesting?
- joseph henrich
Yeah. So this begins with a paper by my colleague Ben Enke in economics at Harvard. And what Ben did was he went to this — there’s a psychologist named Jon Haidt, who’s been collecting data at yourmorals.org, which you can go in there, you answer a bunch of questions, and it kind of puts you on a place within a five-dimensional morality scheme.
And those dimensions are cool. But to simplify things, you can simplify this down to one big and most important dimension which runs from moral universalism where people are seeing others as more or less equal. The in group, out group difference is minimized. People care about a lot of harm and fairness, not so much about hierarchy and loyalty and some of these other dimensions of morality. Down to the more parochial end, where people have a smaller circle of cooperation and they’re very concerned about loyalties and hierarchy.
So you get this spectrum. And you can measure that at the U.S. county level because the data set is rich enough. And what Ben is able to show — and this is published in a top econ journal. It’s a very reliable result. A bunch of back up studies and stuff — is that counties that were more morally parochial were more likely to vote for Donald Trump. And it was that vote total over and above what Mitt Romney or John McCain got. So it’s the sort of extra Trump boost from being high on moral parochialism.
Well, we wanted to know if that would replicate for 2020, and that seemed to be the case there. And then we tried to explain the variation. So there’s this county-level variation. And the first thing to note is that if you plot from 2008 to 2019, what you see is a divergence. So the urban areas maintain, maybe they go up a little bit in moral universalism, whereas the rural counties are going down the whole time, declining in their moral universalism, getting more parochial, OK? So we have temporal variation.
And then the question is, can we explain that?
And the two factors that came out of our first set of analyzes is, first, shocks of any kind. So economic shocks seem to make people more morally parochial, and weather shocks. So climate change hit some U.S counties more than other U.S. counties. And if you get hit with a shock, hurricane, say, flood, fire, you get a bit more morally parochial.
And the final thing that seemed to matter — and we’re not sure how to deal with this — is that movement. Americans have been moving less and less, and they’re staying put more. But places that stayed disproportionately more, meaning most people stayed where they were born, seem to also be more morally parochial.
And that makes sense with this kind of distant ties idea that you move to a new place and you’ve got to make new friends and you create new social networks. If you stay in the same place, you kind of establish long-term family and friend ties and whatnot. And that works towards more morally parochialism. So that could be related to the economic shocks and stuff, and the sort of directions of the causal hours there that’s causing us some challenges.
- ezra klein
One obvious trend we seem to be seeing over the past 100 years, past 50, I think past 20 is the world is getting weirder, right? This directionality seems to be happening in a lot of places. Does that become a worrying monoculture at some point? It was a dimension of cultural evolution. But if it becomes everywhere, do you get a situation where there’s not enough diversity, where we’re, as a species, overindexed on individualism or something like that?
Do you worry about the world getting too weird? Is it plausible the competitive advantage will be going in the other direction, sort of like the Mormons in America? Do you see something happening here where what’s been a competitive advantage will become a disadvantage, or at least something that is more widespread than would probably be optimal?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, this is a very common question. And what’s behind the question is that there’s this linear trajectory that we’ve been going on. And I guess I think that what I’m seeing — and I don’t have any quantitative data to offer you. But as what were European institutions, take universities or Democratic government, has spread to all these different places. In some cases, it’s a copy of the U.S. Constitution is spread widely. But then it has to interface with the existing local institutions.
So if you just take we were talking about China and Japan before. I think what’s emerging there is somewhat weirder psychology, but the societies are often also going off in new directions. So think of something more complicated than a two-dimensional line we’re going along. So a one-dimensional line, but rather there’s different ways of going. So I don’t think there’s really a kind of unilineal thing going on here. I think or something more complicated going on.
And what we need to do is just do the more psychological work to really map the space of all the different possibilities and see where things are going. To what degree — in China, there are some capitalist-type institutions there are some free markets. But, obviously, at the political level, there’s not democracy. But, although, at the lower level, there is democracy. So it’s complicated and we haven’t sorted it all out yet.
- ezra klein
Well, when you look on psychological surveys, how are people differing? When you say something new is emerging, are we able to see on the same surveys where people are showing a different set of preferences or tendencies?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so one example that makes China challenging — and this kind of illustrates the bigger problem — is that if you ask people to generalize trust question, which is the survey, they seem very trusting. But then if you ask the more detailed set of questions — do you trust foreigners, do you trust people you met for the first time versus people who live around you — and you analyze those, they don’t look very trusting.
So in Australia and the U.S., if you ask people the generalized trust question, it goes along with, it’s correlated with that in group, out group trust I was describing. Whereas in China, they seem to be unrelated. So it’s as if life is structuring itself in a way so that most people you interact with, you trust. But if you ask me about foreigners, I don’t trust those people. So that’s the more complicated picture. And I haven’t seen any analyses yet that look for the kind of multidimensional shape that I’m talking about.
- ezra klein
I think that is a good place to come to an end. So always our final question, what are the three books you would recommend to the audience?
- joseph henrich
Well, let’s see. I really like “Why Europe?” by Michael Mitterauer if you wanted to learn more about Europe. I still think everybody should read Jared Diamond’s “Guns, Germs, and Steel.” And I really like this book, “The Chosen Few: How Education Shaped Jewish History,” because it really emphasizes how early changes in a religion of a particular group had big, long-term consequences.
- ezra klein
Joe Henrich, thank you very much.
- joseph henrich
Great to be with you. [MUSIC PLAYING]
This episode of “The Ezra Klein Show” was produced by Rogé Karma. Fact checking by Michelle Harris, Mary Marge Locker, and Kate Sinclair. Mixing by Sonia Herrero. Our production team is Emefa Agawu, Annie Galvin, Jeff Geld, Rogé Karma, and Kristin Lin. Original music by Isaac Jones. Audience strategy by Shannon Busta. The executive producer of New York Times Opinion Audio is Annie-Rose Strasser. And special thanks to Kristina Samulewski. [MUSIC PLAYING]
If You’re Reading This, You’re Probably ‘WEIRD’
The anthropologist Joseph Henrich parses how culture shapes our psyches.
transcript
If You’re Reading This, You’re Probably ‘WEIRD’
The anthropologist Joseph Henrich parses how culture shapes our psyches.
[MUSIC PLAYING]
- ezra klein
So here’s the thing. If you’re listening to this podcast, you’re pretty WEIRD. You’re probably very weird, and not just for all the obvious reasons you’re thinking of. In social science, or at least certain corners of it, WEIRD is now an acronym. It stands for a certain kind of person: western, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic.
And WEIRD people, who have been the people we’ve been surveying and studying for a lot of research on psychology, they actually turn out to be different, much more so than they, than we often realize or admit. There are all these things we take for granted as basic elements of human psychology and ethics that are actually peculiar to the WEIRD psychology.
We take them for granted because we feel them. We take them for granted because we study ourselves and then use that to extrapolate to human nature, but we shouldn’t. The idea that we have a stable self that exists across all contexts, that a person’s intentions should be central to any evaluation of their actions, that guilt is a widely felt emotion, that self-esteem is crucial for happiness, we treat all these as truisms, but they’re not.
At least that’s the argument made by Joseph Henrich. Henrich is an anthropologist at Harvard who has done really deep, rich cross-cultural research in how different forms of human culture shape our psychologies, and into what those psychologies actually are. His 2015 book “The Secret of our Success” argued that what sets human beings apart from other species is our capacity for cultural learning.
His 2020 book “The Weirdest People in the World” takes that argument and extends it, arguing that beginning sometime in the Middle Ages, certain cultural and, really, religious shifts radically transformed the psychologies of individuals living in Europe. And that, then, the emergence of this WEIRD psychology was a prerequisite to everything from the development of market economies to representative government to human rights.
It’s a really fascinating argument. And if you take it seriously, it says something really quite profound about the indirect and unusual ways that human beings and human cultures evolve.
As always, my email: [email protected].
[MUSIC PLAYING]
Joe Henrich, welcome to the show.
- joseph henrich
It’s good to be with you.
- ezra klein
So the premise of your book, is that you, and me, and basically all the people listening to this podcast, and virtually all the people in the studies on which we have all based our ideas about what people are like are a little weird or a little distinctive? Tell me how.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so the main thing is that a lot of what you read in a psychology textbook or any of your typical psychology papers come from sampling one particular population. And as psychologists, and anthropologists, and economists began to measure psychology around the world, we found a great deal of variation along things like individualism, the relevance of shame versus guilt, the importance of analytic versus holistic thinking, the role of intentionality and things like moral judgment, and a number of other areas — time thrift, temporal discounting, and I could keep going. But there’s this interesting pattern of global variation in how people think about the world.
- ezra klein
I want to go through some specific pieces of this, or maybe run a very quick experiment with the audience. So I want to go through a bunch of different pieces of this, but let me start by having us all run a little exercise here. So if you say to yourself the words “I am,” what fills in the blank? Just take a second.
So, for me, I think of things like I’m a journalist. I’m a Californian. But there’s a study on this you write about, Joe. And I was wondering if you could talk through the “I Am” study.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, it’s called the 20 Statements Test. And they ask people to fill in the I am and then blank, or the who am I is another way of approaching it. And they just look at all the things that people respond. And so when you do this with populations in the U.S., say, people say things like I’m smart, I’m a kayaker, I’m curious. All these kind of things that relate to their attributes and their accomplishments and their aspirations. So things about themselves as an individual.
But when you do this in other places, people very quickly translate the question into things about their relationships. So I might say, I am a father, I am a brother, I’m a member of a certain group. And those tend to dominate it. So you get many more things about relationships as opposed to things about attributes, accomplishments, and aspirations. So this has to do with how people think about themselves. Am I a node in a relational network, or am I a unitary thing with my own unique attributions and ways of approaching the world?
- ezra klein
And how big are these differences? Because, obviously, there’s going to be some overlap in the curve here.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, at the extremes, American undergraduates will give you no relationships. They never mentioned that they’re a child or something like that, a son or something. And then if you go somewhere like the Maasai in Kenya, they’ll give you almost all relational attributes. So it runs the gamut of the spectrum. But, of course, lots of places are somewhere in the middle.
- ezra klein
What is a place that is in the middle? And not just on this, but in a bunch of the things we’ll talk about. When we think about the poles these different personality typologies can be, what is a place that seems to be more balanced between them than others?
- joseph henrich
Right. The famous psychologist Richard Nisbett started doing comparisons between immigrants from Asia that were attending the University of Michigan, and then European-descent Americans at the University of Michigan. And he was finding differences. So then people started studying Japan, and China, and Korea, places like that. And so it was thought that there was this big difference between Asia, the East, and the West.
But it turns out that for lots of things, Asia is actually somewhere in the middle. And you’ve got to go to places like the Maasai in Kenya or somewhere like that, where you have completely different social structure and whatnot to get the full range of difference.
- ezra klein
So in the book, you call at least part of this psychological profile that we’re talking about in the West the individualism complex. Tell me what that is.
- joseph henrich
Right. So for a long time, psychologists and anthropologists have been talking about this individualism complex. And I think at the core of it is the notion that we think of ourselves as a unitary selves and not as a node in a relational network. And that tends to have clustering around it — things like overconfidence, a reliance on guilt versus shame, a tendency of self-enhancement — so putting your best foot forward, emphasizing your attributes, and suppressing your deficits or deficiencies. Things like that.
- ezra klein
And you make the point that these different psychological approaches, they don’t just emerge for no reason, that there are ways of navigating and succeeding in different cultural institutions. So what is the individualism complex adaptive to? What would make it be the thing that we would select for?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, that’s a great way of putting the question. The way to think about it is, in the world that individualists are trying to adapt to, there are small families. And most of your relationships are optional and potentially ephemeral. So there’s a marketplace for finding friends, business partners, and marriage partners. The individual has a lot of choice in this. And you’re cultivating a unique self, so you’re trying to emphasize those traits which will make you interesting to possible friends, possible mates, and possible business partners. So it could be honesty, intelligence. These traits tend to be dispositional in the sense that they are operative across context. So when someone says you’re honest, they don’t mean you’re just honest with your friends and then dishonest everybody else. The suggestion is is that you have this trait that stretches across lots of different kinds of interactions.
Whereas, in other places, you’re born into a network, and you get, by virtue of your birth, lots of responsibilities, lots of social connections, definitions about who you’re likely to marry. There could be arranged marriages involved. So, really, what your job is is to figure out how to prosper within this prebuilt network. If you need to set up a business partnership, for example, you’ll look not for someone who’s trustworthy and smart, which you might do in the other world, but instead, you’re looking for someone who is connected to you through lots of social ties because those social ties will make that person trustworthy.
- ezra klein
You just touched on something that I found fascinating when I read about it, which is that when we talk about people in the WEIRD basket, there’s more of a sense of stability in our sense of self. I’m curious to hear you expand on that a bit. How stable, how unstable is this in other places, and why would stability be prized, particularly in a situation where you have more choice? You might think in a situation with more choices, and more options, and more capacity to move between different kinds of institutions and groups and people, that it would select for immutability, right? You can be this here and that there. That it doesn’t isn’t intuitive to me, so I’m curious why you think it is.
- joseph henrich
The key idea is that there could be reputational effects. And in the individualistic world, a lot of people are in the same category, sort of like strangers who you interact with who you might turn out to have a profitable interaction with, mutually beneficial. Could be a friendship, could be a business partnership. And so you want to be known as someone who’s honest or someone who’s intelligent across all these different contexts.
Whereas, in the relational world, you’ll have some kinds of cousins, for example, that you have a, quote, joking relationship. So anthropologists have documented the joking relationship between what they call cross cousins, which is when your parents are opposite sex. And that’s a very funny, playful relationship. But then with other kinds of cousins, the same genetic distance, you’ll have to have a relationship of respect. You’ll have to defer to them in conversation. There’s no joking around. The same thing you might have your father, and then your father’s brother. If it’s an older brother, you have to really defer to that older brother. And you wouldn’t speak in his presence, and you take his orders, and that kind of thing. So you just have to be mutable across these different contexts. How you’re going to behave with your professor is quite different than how you’re going to behave with your friends.
And you can see that flattening increasing probably even in our lifetime. So my undergraduates show up in my office at Harvard and say, hey, Joe, how’s it going? That gives you a sense of the just general stranger category.
- ezra klein
Why do you think we have seen that flattening? I mean, this had gotten a lot flatter in America than it’s been in other places. I take your point that it is getting flatter still. There seems to be a general trend against almost any kind of formality, right? I just moved away from San Francisco.
But something that has always struck me there is how much the fashion is flattened. The very, very rich people just shop at REI and wear a lot of Patagonia and REI-branded vests. And this goes back to Bill Gates, and Mark Zuckerberg, and hoodies. But there is something there about trying to, at least aesthetically, collapse distinctions and create a kind of flatness that would not have been even seen as desirable in other times. Whether it’s true, I think we can argue that. I don’t think it is true, but the desire to make it seem true is very present.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, and I think that’s just the spreading of this notion of egalitarian individualism. We’re all just individuals. We have these traits and we can interact freely, and we don’t have to be scared or anything of other people. Whereas in lots of societies, the very structure and organization of the society is rooted in hierarchy and respect for authority.
And that has a bunch of downstream implications. It means that it’s easier to come to decisions. Politics works a little bit differently. But you might not go into business with someone, for example, that is very hierarchical at a different place than you, for example.
- ezra klein
You make a distinction between self-esteem and other esteem. Tell me about that.
- joseph henrich
Yeah. So in an individualistic society, one of the things you’re trying to do is cultivate a set of traits that will make you stand out that other people will find appealing as well. So you might have a rule, for example, that you go to the gym every day. And if you don’t go to the gym, you might feel guilty. And that could affect your self-esteem because you’re not living up to the traits that you think.
But that doesn’t induce shame. Other people don’t judge you for that necessarily. Your brother doesn’t — people don’t look down on him because you failed to go to the gym or abide by some of your other norms.
Whereas in other societies, there’s a much more shared set of rules, and things like shame affect your close relative. So if you do something really shameful, then it actually makes your family feel shame, and it lowers their reputational standards.
So there do seem to be these big differences. So the relationship between self-esteem and happiness and stuff seems to be very important in WEIRD populations, but not so important in other places. So a lot of what we think of as good psychology, clinical psychology is actually a kind of WEIRD psychology.
- ezra klein
I was really struck by this that you write that, quote, “In the few non-WEIRD societies where it has been studied, having high self-esteem and a positive view of oneself are not strongly linked to either life satisfaction or subjective well-being.” And I want to hold on that for a minute because it gets at something that I think is really profound inside of your work, which is not just that in different cultures, people act differently, or rate different things, or have a different answer to the question of I am, but that when you do shift them along these dimensions, the way you experience the world actually might be pretty perceptually different.
I think to try to inhabit the idea that having high self-esteem, thinking highly of yourself, would not affect how you felt about you or your life is pretty strange, right? That’s actually a bigger perceptual gap than I think most of us think about day-to-day. So what do you make of that finding?
Having done a lot of this work, how do you think about the contingency or distinctiveness of your own perspective? How different do you think it would be to experience a world from within the cognitive structure of some of the societies you’ve studied, or, going back into the past, some of those that you have read about?
- joseph henrich
Yeah. I think that there’s a way we can get inside of this. There’s a lot of talk in our society about cultivating your true self, and finding your passion, and this kind of thing. And if you grow up in a world where the real emphasis and the thing that everybody was supposed to do was cultivate their family, care for their elders, and people took real pride in child rearing, and social connections, and strengthening their family over time, the things that might make you happy would be fulfilling all those culturally acquired goals as opposed to achieving some kind of personal state of being unique, and special, and having set yourself apart from others. So I just see it as having gotten different kinds of goals early on.
- ezra klein
So there’s another divergence between WEIRD and non-WEIRD populations that I think is interesting here, which is the degree to which we take into account people’s intentions when we judge their actions. Can you give me a few examples of how that might differ?
- joseph henrich
So the way we measure this is with a simple anecdote. So we have a simple story that we give people where someone is at a busy market. They have a bag with a few items in it. And in one story, they put the bag down to look at some items that are displayed on a counter, and someone else comes up and takes the bag and disappears off. And then by varying the story in different ways, we can make it clear that the person had a very similar bag, which they put down right next to it, and then they accidentally picked up the wrong bag. Or a case where it’s clearly a theft and they were trying to make off with the goods.
And then we asked people questions about how blameworthy the person is, how much they should be punished, things like that. And what we find is that we go all the way from WEIRD societies where it’s all about the intention. Really, people want to kind of forgive the guy who made a mistake. All the way down to there’s no difference. The person is out their goods one way or another.
Another way to think about this, if you accidentally burn someone’s house down or if you intentionally burn someone’s house down, the bottom line is the person has no house. So it just turns out that how important those intentions are, those mental states and — you can do it with beliefs or intentions — really varies across societies from places where it’s not important at all to places where it’s super important. It’s probably the most important single factor.
- ezra klein
This predicts a bit where we’re going to go in this conversation, but a big theme of your work is about how these differing temperaments, psychologies end up netting out in cultural and governmental and societal institutions. And so how do societies that differ dramatically on this question of intentions — how do their legal, or retributive, or judicial systems end up differing?
- joseph henrich
Yeah. And so you can see this in the history of European law, where there’s a lot of discussion and logic chopping and labels for different mental states that characters might have when doing something that could be a crime. Did they intend to do it? Did they think the thing that they did could do it? So there’s all these ways that lawyers break down the mental states of the actors. And each of these has implications for the nature and details of the crime, and the degree of culpability, and the punishment, all these kinds of things.
But lots of societies have just done the simple thing. If you go back and you look at pre-Christian law codes, if you accidentally shoot a guy with an arrow, then you have to pay a certain blood price to his family. And nobody cares if the arrow glanced off of a deer or if you were just trying to kill him. And then you see this in lots of other societies where intentionality either plays no role or a smaller role than the sort of obsession over the details of the mental states.
And just to make it clear that this is also part of religion. So if you get to Protestantism, it’s all about faith, right? You can somehow get to heaven by just having faith. Faith alone. No good works matters. Whereas the Catholics are more about the good works and the faith together, right? So Protestantism is kind of extreme on how important this is. In other religions, people don’t talk a lot about mental states. It’s really like, are you doing the rituals? Are you part of the community? Are you engaged? That kind of thing.
- ezra klein
I think there can be a tendency to look at the evolution, the selection towards these psychologies or caring about intentions, or whatever it might be, as almost teleological, right? We are evolving towards a higher moral state. The arc of the universe is bending towards a kind of moral justice.
I don’t think you see it that way. And so make the case for me on some of these. For instance, the relational dynamics, or the intentional dynamics, or taking into account of attention the ways in which our particular way of looking at this may not be some better way of doing it necessarily, just different, right? When you talk to people in these societies and you try to explain the way you see it or the way the place you come from sees it, what do they tell you? What do you hear that you find convincing?
- joseph henrich
So one of the things that always sticks in my mind, I was talking — I do work in these remote islands in Fiji. And I was discussing with this old Fijian elder why it is when they have a council meeting to make decisions about how things are going to happen in the village, that only a small number of elders has a discussion with the chief. Everybody else just sits quietly in the background. And then that small committee of old guys will make a decision without any input from anybody else in the room.
And I said, why not do majority vote? Everyone could just vote, and you can make a decision that way. Naturally, I thought of that because of where I come from. And he was like, forget about women, right? We only got as far as discussing what the men might do. He says, well, how can an 18-year-old who has no children and has never taken care of the clan or the family have the same weigh-in on this decision as I have, someone who has grandchildren and who has taken care of this big, very successful clan? Those things just don’t seem like they should be the same, right? So there’s a certain logic to that. That kind of makes sense.
- ezra klein
Tell me about the passengers dilemma experiment.
- joseph henrich
So this is a kind of vignette-type experiment from sociology. And in the experiment, you’re in a car, and your friend is there. Now, you can vary — you can make it a family member, too. But in the basic version, it’s a friend. The friend is driving too fast for the conditions, a little bit recklessly, and they hit someone and kill them. And then a law case is going to occur. And your friend’s lawyer says, if you testify that your friend was going under the speed limit that he’ll get off, it’ll be fine, nobody else was there. Or you can tell the truth in court. And I’ve even presented this in class. And my classes are pretty diverse.
Some members of the class can’t believe that anybody would tell the truth in court. But this varies tremendously across populations. With Canadians, 90 percent of Canadians will tell the truth in court. At least that’s what they say they would do. Whereas in other places, it would be crazy to tell the truth in the court. You’ve got to help your friend. Your friend needs you. Aren’t you a good friend? And so those are two virtues that are trading off. But how you trade those virtues off has a big effect on the effectiveness of legal institutions, right?
- ezra klein
This is a place to go back to the question I asked a second ago, where I think the non-WEIRD response actually makes a lot of intuitive sense. In this particular case, you’re trading off the interest in a way of this faceless, impersonal institution — the legal system — over someone you care about. It’s not a case where they did something where — to go back to the point of intention very deeply — they weren’t trying to do this maliciously, right? They made a mistake anybody can make.
And so you can kind of, I think, see not just the rationality, but, in some very deep way, the morality of siding with those who are nearest to you, right? Siding with the people you care about. How, as you understand it, does that evolution away from that happen? Because I do think that would have been the much more normal reaction almost all through history. And then, eventually, you get to get to Canada. How do you get to Canada?
- joseph henrich
Well, one of the things that I try to push in the book, or try to really press on, is this idea of an interpersonal pro-sociality or morality versus an interpersonal. So one is this reality you have with strangers towards arbitrary rules, whether it’s paying your taxes, giving blood to strangers. All these things that help make society run. But it’s really kind of faceless, and you’re not really helping anybody you know. Versus the kind of more normal human morality where I got friends and family, and I want to do stuff to help them, and those are my priority.
Now, of course, everybody has that interpersonal morality. But it’s a question of how much emphasis you’re putting on one and how that you’re going to make the trade offs versus that general principle, don’t lie in court that’s required to make the system run. People can’t be always lying in court. So it’s that kind of trade-off. And then much of my book is about trying to lay out how that unfolded over centuries to get us to the place where we are today. And you shouldn’t think of today as an endpoint. I think things are dynamic, and directions are changing, and that sort of thing.
- ezra klein
Let’s talk a bit about that space of evolution, though. I’ll keep going through some of the dimensions of WEIRDness. But I am interested here because something your book pains to argue is we have a bit of — I think it’s fair to call it now a myth in WEIRD societies that what caused all this was our devotion to higher order, enlightened principles, impersonal rules. We care about markets and codes of ethics and ethical philosophy.
And I think you show pretty conclusively — and we’ll get into the way family structures changed. But I think you show pretty conclusively that religion has been just this unbelievably powerful driver of cultural evolution and, arguably, is behind a lot of the personality changes that made things the enlightenment, like believing in something like the Bill of Rights, possible. So tell me a bit about the role of religion and what you might call non-rational principles in getting us to this place where impersonal structures and principles can be so dominant.
- joseph henrich
So the first is kind of an anthropological observation that if we look at the database of religions and different societies, many societies have relatively weak, whimsical — they might have a morality like a person, but it looks quite different than the big, moralizing gods that we would find in Islam or Christianity and religions like that. So I’ve long been interested in how the beliefs in these more powerful, more moralizing gods emerged.
And myself and lots of collaborators have made the case that societies that had beliefs in these more powerful, moralizing gods were able to galvanize more cooperation in larger groups. So if you believe that God was incentivizing you with, say, heaven and hell, then that can lead people to behave in somewhat more pro-social ways and allow trade, mutually beneficial transactions, larger cooperation, cooperation and warfare.
And so simple experiments that we’ve done is — one is just to go around the world to different societies. And we’re going to remote villages in Africa and Fiji and New Guinea and stuff and just asking people questions, getting measurements of their degree to which they believe in a god that is moralizing and punishing that has control over the afterlife.
And we find that if we give people a choice between allocating money in a way that the experimenter can’t be sure what they did to their community or themselves versus some coreligionist distant stranger who they don’t know, they’re more fair. They’re not totally fair, but they’re more fair towards the stranger in this monetary allocation when they report believing in these more powerful, moralizing gods.
And then as a kind of addendum to that, you can take people who do believe in the moralizing gods and unconsciously slip them cues of religion or of their god and then have them do these monetary allocations that economists like. And then they’re more generous, or they’re more adhering to the pro-social norms. More equal divisions with the distant stranger when we’ve unconsciously slipped them cues. There’s also cool natural experiments where you can use the call to prayer. And the call of prayer gives them a little boost. They’re a little more prosocial after the call to prayer.
[MUSIC PLAYING]
- ezra klein
Would it be fair — to gloss your view of human beings here — to say that you believe much less, I think, in a human nature than is, I think, the culturally dominant story and what you tend to believe in is much more of a cultural nature, that we are actually quite plastic under different cultural equilibriums? And as such, can be moved, and changed, and shifted, and formed into many more shapes as human beings depending on what culture we grow up in then I think a lot of us appreciate now.
- joseph henrich
Yeah, I think that is a fair gloss. The only place where I’d want to shape that a bit would be I teach human nature as a general education requirement at Harvard. And the thing I emphasize in that class is that we’re a cultural species, that our reliance on this cultural learning is actually part of our evolved phenotype. So we’ve genetically evolved to come into the world as babies and start imitating and acquiring and using cues to figure out who to pay attention to, and drinking in all this.
So we have a lot of extra brain. And we have changes in our developmental cycle which allow us to acquire social norms during middle childhood, say, ages 5 to 10. We internalize them. So we use them to help us navigate the world. We’re willing to pay costs if the norm demands those costs. But that can lead to all kinds of different ways of thinking about the world because even something like whether we see a visual illusion depends on the world we construct.
So one of my favorite experiments are these the Müller-Lyer illusion, which is the two arrows where one of the arrows are in and the other of the arrows are out. Well, if you grow up in a world without carpenter corners — and anthropologists have done the work in the 1960s — you actually don’t see that illusion. So you literally see the world differently. And I think that captures a lot of some of the stuff we were talking about earlier with the role of self-esteem and stuff. You literally see the world differently.
- ezra klein
You have a great line here where you write, you can’t separate culture from psychology or psychology from biology because culture physically rewires our brains, and thereby shapes how we think. Your example there with the carpenters corners is, I think, a pretty good example of that. So how does culture rewire our brains?
- joseph henrich
The idea is that we come in with a degree of plasticity in order to help us acquire the information processing that allows us to navigate the institutional landscapes, the incentives that are built up in the institutions. So just my favorite example and the one I start the book off with is when you learn to read, you get specialized neural circuitry in your left ventral hemisphere. It impinges on some of your facial processing.
So literate people are right-biased in their facial processing, but nonliterate people are much more symmetrical in their facial processing. So what neuroscientists thought was a product of humans, a right bias in the brain in terms of processing faces, turns out to be a product of literacy because you spend all this time as a child building this machine that can read. It reads automatically. One of the fun things I say in the book is that if I show you a word, you can’t stop yourself from reading it.
Even though this is just a learned skill, it’s a thing that most cultures didn’t do over human history, but now it’s an automatic piece of our brain. It takes up neurogeography. It affects things that have nothing to do with reading, so you process speech differently once you’ve learned to read. We have a thicker corpus callosum. So we know the actual biology is different. But this is all due to this cultural value on reading.
- ezra klein
So let me then use this as the bridge over to this other set of ideas. I kind of think of you as known for two big ideas. One is WEIRD cultures, the WEIRD personality type. And, obviously, there are many other co-authors on that and everything. But the other, and in some ways a part of your work, that has shaped my thinking more is around cultural evolution.
So people are very familiar with individual evolution. The idea that there is a selection pressure and so much in who we can be traced back to what gave us biological fitness to reproduce. What is cultural evolution? How do cultures evolve? What are the pressures on them that help them do that? And then, how does that then differ from individual evolution?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so I come at this by thinking about how natural selection would have shaped our genes to give us minds that allowed us to acquire information from other people. So it begins with the individual and how they can adaptively learn, when they should learn from the group, say, some kind of conformist effect, when they should attend to prestigious people, how they should integrate information from different people.
But if everyone’s doing that over generations, you can get products. And those products can be technologies that are so complex that no individual could figure out them in their lifetime. And, eventually, people don’t even know how it works. They just use it, and it does work.
Another thing would be an institution. So institutions long predate written laws. So something like marriage that’s just a cluster of social norms about who can marry who, what the relative responsibilities are in the marriage, who pays who, that kind of thing. But then the social structure, everybody who comes after that has to grow up in this world where the social networks have been organized by some norms about who marries who and where people have to live after they get married.
So in that sense, it creates this cultural niche that people’s minds have to adapt to. And I make the case that this has been going on for over a million years and has shaped much of our genetic evolution. So our genes have had to respond to the fact that we create these cultural products.
The simplest example is fire and cooking. So there’s good evidence that that goes back pretty far. But if you look at our physiology — our stomachs and our large intestines, our teeth and stuff — we’re an animal that’s dependent on cooked food. Cooking DNA, which is the proteins. It breaks things down. Essentially, its digestion outside the body. And so it means there’s much less need for all the digestive tissue that we see in other apes. So if you think about other apes, they have this big spreading rib cage that comes out, and they have these large stomachs. That’s actually for their large intestines to give them extra space to break down fibers, and process meat, and things like that. But humans don’t have that because we cook the stuff, and then we do the digestion outside. So it’s shaped our physiology, but it’s strictly — none of us know how to make fire innately. This has to be learned from growing up in a particular place. So it’s a simple example of gene culture co-evolution. But I think that social norms, and institutions, and the creation of languages has also fed back and shaped aspects of our genetic evolution.
- ezra klein
Walk me through how human children develop compared to newborns in other species.
- joseph henrich
The first thing is that — biological anthropologists study this. And it looks like children are born premature in the sense that very young human babies are helpless and they seem to come out too early. And the main reason many think that they come out too early is that their heads are getting too big. So we have a basic primate body design. And if that head gets too big, it can’t get out through the uterus and whatnot.
And so babies come out before that head gets too big. And then the baby head continues expanding and it can be taken care of. And humans have all these extra parents, these alloparents that help. And food sharing, all that kind of stuff creates this environment where you can raise a very altricial, helpless baby in this kind of world.
But humans also have this unique period after about age five or something called middle childhood. And that’s where we have quite big brains, but we still have little bodies. And this seems to be the period when kids learn all of this stuff while growing up. So it looks like selection has monkeyed around with gestation. And then we have very short nursing periods, or shorter nursing periods than other animals, probably because we can break down the food with cooking and process in other ways.
And then we have this new period, middle childhood, where we learn all the norms and get a lot of the details of the culture. And then, finally, when we have that, then we get the body expansion. And then adolescence you can think of as apprenticeship time. So adolescence, you start doing adult-like things, working with adults, eventually getting up into proficiency by the time you’re in your 20s.
Anthropologists studying hunter-gatherers, you don’t really even get the best hunters — this is thinking about the male occupation usually — until, say, mid-30s, 40s or whatnot. So there’s a lot of knowledge that you have to learn in order to be the most competent person in the community.
- ezra klein
So we have these genes. We have these aptitudes. We have these capabilities. But we don’t — I read a lot of sci-fi that has some kind of mechanistic way of moving one culture’s ideas into the next generation, right? You stick a computer chip or a download into an A.I., or read a book about spiders where they sort of somehow encode it in a sack. But we don’t have that.
And so the societies that have out-competed each other and that sort of have won this cultural evolution game, at least for now or for a period, have developed what are fundamentally cultural technologies that have helped them cooperate and pass knowledge around. And a lot of these in your book seem to be encoded in religion. And you have this view, I think, of religion as having, at its explicit level, a story about god and/or gods, and how the world works, and moralities.
And then beneath that, a set of things like ritual, and collective dance, and other things that are really incredibly powerful technologies of cultural transmission. So can you tell me about how those two things interact? Why are the structures of religion such an important part of your book and an important part of this cultural evolution story?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so it really comes from — my background, Ph.D., is in anthropology. And when you look across diverse human societies, you don’t see the need partitioning that we tend to think about the world. A typical partitioning where we have the political scientists study politics, the economists study economics. There’s some theologians and stuff who take care of religion. But in other societies, the religion is woven throughout the production system. And then there’s rituals associated with production, which is part of exchange.
All of this is justified by mystical beliefs and reified in rituals, which people do. And we now know that rituals actually have a big effect on bonding people. And whether it’s the marching together or moving together in synchrony, or listening to music together, or just doing it repeatedly seems to have these interesting psychological effects. So societies have evolved and figured out ways to do that.
And, as you mentioned, one of the features of cultural evolution that is probably not an important feature of genetic evolution for humans is this competition among societies. So you get this sorting of different institutions. And so you get production systems and distribution systems, but they’re interwoven with religion. And religion makes them do better.
So one of the societies that I discuss in the book is the Arapesh from New Guinea. And there, if things are going poorly for the village, they’ll re-initiate some rituals. But they think they’re doing it to appease the god — their god — who they think is causing bad stuff to happen because he’s not happy. Now this often leads to good things happening because they’ve done the ritual and they’ve rebonded, and now they’re cooperating again.
So they got kind of the wrong theory about — at least in my view — they have the wrong theory about the world, but it causes them to do something that’s really helpful, which is kind of redo the rituals, rebond, and begin cooperating again as a group. So this process has affected the evolution of religions, and eventually gives us the big, powerful moralizing gods that we’re all familiar with.
- ezra klein
I’ve heard you talk about a study about which kinds of communes — and I happen to be very interested in communes — survived and thrived, or at least lasted for longer periods of time in the 20th century, and how it maps on to this question of religion as a bonding force. Can you talk about that?
- joseph henrich
So in the United States, there was a great awakening and a proliferation of communes. Some of the communes were religious and some weren’t. And then you can use this as a laboratory and then ask the question, which one lasts the longest and has the most number of members?
Well, one group that clearly won was the Mormons. And then there’s also a group called the Hutterites. Other groups like the Shakers had a norm where you weren’t allowed to have sex, right? The group could only grow by in-migration. That one didn’t make it. And, notably, the ones that were religious in context were the ones that had the longest duration. So the ones that were just a bunch of political ideologues getting together and forming a group, they tended to sputter out, and they didn’t win the long race.
- ezra klein
I took your book as being fundamentally pretty pessimistic on two things that I think are fairly widely believed right now. One is the ability to change cultures dramatically just by telling them to change. But almost every major story of change I could think of from the book was pretty indirect. You created a lot of cooperation, usually around a religious story. And that ended up having these very profound downstream impacts. Whereas the much more straightforward, we’re all going to do this because it’s the right thing to do and we’re a bunch of rationalists who’ve decided to live together in this way, it doesn’t seem to have a lot of staying power.
And so downstream from that, I think it would be intuitive for a lot of secular WEIRD people to believe they’re probably much better at shaping societies more profoundly and potently because we’re free of all this superstition and Kant and WEIRDness. But, in fact, it seems to me a lesson potentially in the book is it’s actually going to be much harder for societies to evolve if they don’t have the cooperative power provided by belief and rituals that emerge from belief in something higher, that there seems to be some kind of capacity of that to get people to do the somewhat illogical, very difficult things, like, say, don’t eat pork or take the Sabbath off, to talk about Jewish examples, but you can pick them from whatever religion you want, that end up bonding a community together at the level that allows them to achieve really great things.
Is that your view that a lot of this is indirect and that we will lose something pretty profound if we continue to secularize in terms of our ability to have this kind of cultural evolution?
- joseph henrich
I definitely agree with the first part. This kind of enlightenment notion that we built the world through rationality. And if we could just convince people to be rational, they will. So that I’m definitely pessimistic on. And I think there’s tons of examples from economic development to all kinds of things that support that.
One of the things that I’m interested in is whether we can harvest insights from religion, use of ritual, things like that to improve secular impersonal institutions, build better organizations, that kind of thing. So I’m not in favor of spreading supernatural agents because we think it’ll be effective, but I think there’s wisdom embedded in religious traditions that maybe is underused in impersonal secular institutions.
- ezra klein
I want to press on this for a minute because I don’t quite buy what you just said. I don’t 100 percent believe that you buy it because so many of the examples you give and of the actual research you give has to do with the way really truly complex rituals — I mean, very complex dances, and fasting, and collective movement — and things you just really can’t get people to do. You can’t go to people and say, you should fast once a week, and do a lot of dances, and do a lot of praying and so on, because, ultimately, this maps onto some kind of human synchronicity mechanism that’s going to help you cooperate.
The real lesson I took from the book is that religion, because it gives you a reason to do things that don’t themselves have an obvious logical justification, allows you to band together these cooperation mechanisms that kind of nothing else does.
You can’t be just in a political system — can you imagine Joe Biden coming out to the Democratic Party tomorrow and saying, listen, a really important thing for us Democrats is to work together. And as such, I am asking all of you to stop eating fish three days a week. And once every six months, I would like you all to go to the ocean, and I would like you to grab a bunch of paper and throw it in as a way of expurging your sins. He’s not going to do that because nobody would do it. But it would probably be very good for cooperation among Democrats if they did it. And this just seems to me to be an actually pretty big thing.
I think it’s often talked about, why do Mormons do so much better in America than most other groups? And they have a lot of very complex religious rituals that have sustained in ways that most other religious groups have not been able to sustain the rituals. And as such, there’s a really high level of pro-sociality and cooperation in Utah, much higher than in other places in the country.
And so it does seem to me that something is being lost that probably cannot just be gained through rationalism because it is, in some ways, the illogic and complexity of the thing itself that is working, but nobody’s going to do that precisely because it is so illogical and complex, unless they believe there is some kind of punishment that is indirect for not doing it.
- joseph henrich
The kind of thing I had in mind would be in the service of some notion of — let’s take the U.S. since that’s where we are. It would be in some notion of some kind of set of American commitments, principles — we have lots of principles, lots of commitments — which would be like national service. So you graduate high school. Everybody does either military or national service, or something like that. It’s a shared sacrifice. You’re together with your fellow Americans. You’re working. Another thing would be the ritual of saying the Pledge of Allegiance at the beginning of the day. If you want to build national identity, you need tools like that. Now, of course, I would immediately caution that if you build too strong an American identity, we kind of need a global identity at the moment. So you have to think about those kinds of things. But there are ways to introduce costly activities. And they would be justified by commitment to truth, justice, and the American way, say, or some large or some supra-U.S. commitment, or something like that. But those kinds of policies at least are ideas.
[MUSIC PLAYING]
- ezra klein
I want to loop back, then, to weirdness because one of the stories that the book is telling is that weirdness, this sort of psychological profile and the world built on top of this psychological profile, which is our governments, and our institutions, and our markets, and so much of it, you really describe this as a world the Catholic Church built. Tell me how the Catholic church built it.
- joseph henrich
I think that the oldest and most important of human institutions is the family. And WEIRD people tend to have a peculiar view of the family because many of us grew up in these monogamous nuclear households that are either loosely or not very connected to other households in the area. But that’s not the story for most of humanity, and, in fact, the kinship unit was the center of production and distribution, and often a religious center with ancestor cults and things like that. Chinese is the classic case.
And what the church unknowingly did, unwittingly did was it adopted a series of prohibitions and prescriptions that dismantled the intensive kinship systems of Europe, and broke them down over centuries into monogamous nuclear families. Now some people will be familiar with the famous incest taboos where, at first, the Catholic church around the sixth century outlaws marriage to first cousins. And then it becomes second cousins. And then it eventually stretches out the sixth cousins, which are so distant that most people — we can’t even track who your sixth cousins are.
So, essentially, a taboo is anybody in your relational circle. It includes affinal relatives, so in-laws, and it includes spiritual relatives. And it forces you to get married to people quite socially distanced from you. And so the idea is this would have begun to rewire the social network. There were also laws about inheritance, where the church wanted the individual to inherit and not the group. So it broke down collective inheritance, which is very common across lots of societies.
And this occurred over centuries. And it actually transformed the church from within, and then the church began developing secular procedures and new law procedures. And these became models, which were then used in secular systems, charter towns. Developed a network of Bishop Ricks, which is one of the ways we’re able to figure this out. We look at how long different regions of Europe were under a particular bishop, and then we look at a lot of things, but one is the contemporary psychology.
- ezra klein
One of the things that emerges from this is a set of more impersonal institutions that you really understand as helping to explain why some regions have developed in the way they have while others haven’t. I thought in many ways the strongest bit of data here is from Italy. Do you mind talking through that?
- joseph henrich
So Italy has long been a puzzle for social scientists because, on the one hand, Italy in the North is famous for the Renaissance and the emergence of the banking industry, and the South is famous for corruption, and the mafia, and whatnot. So what’s going on in Italy?
And what most people don’t realize is that northern Italy was under something called the Carolingian empire. So this is Charlemagne around the year 800. He has this large empire. He’s teamed up with the Pope, and they’re gradually creating the parish system. And one of the things that the Carolingian emperors are concerned about was implementing this marriage and the family program.
So northern Italy gets this high dosage of the marriage and family program, while Southern Italy, some of it’s under the Orthodox Church centered and Byzantine. Islamic rulers are present in places like Sicily. Sardinia seems to be outside of most of this. And so Southern Italy only gets incorporated under the Pope in a sense in part of this whole enterprise much later in history than Northern Italy is.
So what we were able to do is take, say, rates of cousin marriage from 20th century Italy, and we can use that to explain variation and corruption, whether people put money in banks, their kind of general trust, a number of different measures. And so it helps, I think, explain this puzzle that’s Italy.
- ezra klein
So I find all the evidence you present on this so great, actually. I love arguments like this. I love arguments that something we didn’t understand had effects we would have never predicted that led to the world that we now live in. And I also want to maintain some skepticism because in anything like this, we’re sort of working backwards. We can see one part of Italy one way, another one the other way. And now we’re trying to test against data we have.
And there is just a lot of correlational data here. You’re looking at, say, modern day Democratic attitudes or working hours, and then the percentage of Catholic missions in an area 500 years ago. And so I guess one question I have for you is, what is your actual confidence level that this explains a very high level of modern development? And maybe one way of asking it is that, in the past however many decades, we’ve seen a tremendous rise in East Asian countries, right? They have had developmental stories that are sort of unlike any other region in that period.
And would that have been something this would have predicted? When you kind of think of pushing the story forward as opposed to backwards, if you were sitting there in 1950 trying to imagine which countries would escape poverty and then which would be able to escape the middle income trap, would you have said, oh, yeah, South Korea, and Japan, and a couple of these are really going to do it? What is, I guess, your level of confidence that this can explain as much as you’re trying to explain with it?
- joseph henrich
Great question. So we talked a bit about Italy. I could tell you the story about Europe, which will be similar. The rest of Europe. So I think one of the ways to get at these ideas — because if you’re a historian, you could possibly think of all kinds of different contingencies. You could try to think of things that are uncontrolled for that may be relevant.
So what I like to do is let’s test it somewhere else. So there’s data from just the Democratic Republic of Congo. So totally different place. Don’t tell me any stories about particular European rulers or anything particular to Europe. What we have is the city of Kananga where people migrate from villages all around the DRC. And those villages are varying distances from historical Catholic missions.
So if we look at those people today, those from villages that were closer to historical Catholic missions have simpler families, fewer kinship ties, and a more moral universalistic psychology. So they have more of the WEIRD psychology that I think traces to this taking a part of the family.
OK, let’s go to China. We can test it in China because now the church is out of the picture, but the details of the agricultural systems affect the intensity of these kinship systems. So in rice growing parts of China, you get very intensive clans. We can check that with data on clan genealogies going back 1,000 years.
And then we can look and see if that can explain contemporary variation among Han Chinese. So throw out anybody who’s not Han. Just look at Han Chinese. So you get rid of any messy variation. And then we find that people from counties or provinces that have differing amounts of rice growing have the expected changes in psychology.
We can do a similar thing in India. And then, most recently, some colleagues from the University of British Columbia looked at the United States. And so cousin marriage began to pop up in parts of the U.S., and then secular laws outlawed cousin marriage. And so you can see a decline in those subgroups, in those kinds of families that were intermarrying, and then long-term economic prosperity — so higher incomes. So it’s kind of a natural experiment because different states in the U.S. cranked this on at different times. And because we have the continuous U.S census, or at least my colleagues at the University of British Columbia do, they can show these long-term effects.
So there’s just a lot of different converging lines of evidence that help build my confidence that there might be something to this. And then, of course, as a scientist, I’m like, well, what’s the alternative explanation you want to offer for this? And then I want to let those compete.
- ezra klein
And I actually believe there’s definitely something to it. But I wonder how much when we think about the pace of global development, and the sort of weight of what is needed to achieve these institutions and the sort of impersonal nature of them, and the ability to have a society where you’re not operating in high-trust kinship networks that allows for very modern forms of economic cooperation.
And, I guess, one thing that makes me wonder about the weight given to Catholicism here is that it would make me think that development in much of Latin America and Africa would have been stronger than it’s been, particularly around these kinds of institutions and their level of corruption and so on.
Because if you just run a search on what countries have very, very high levels of penetration of the Catholic church, you’ll get a lot of the countries in Latin America and in Africa, but you wouldn’t see that line up all that cleanly with the kinds of markets and institutions that you’re talking about spring from this.
So how do you think about the places where there is a lot of Catholicism, but you don’t seem to be seeing the effects that you’re claiming for it?
- joseph henrich
I want to finish the other half of your question. So 1950 and looking at East Asia, what you see there is the adoption of a lot of Western civil law. So China ends polygyny. It burns the genealogies. It’s trying to destroy its clans. The one child policy, which comes later, that ends all cousins. So if you don’t have cousins, you can’t have cousin marriage.
So they essentially do in a period of about five decades what the church took 500 years to do in terms of completely altering the family structure. So they’re implementing the marriage and family program that the church implemented, and they have the full power of the Chinese state to do it.
And the Japanese actually get started on this first. In 1880, the Meiji Restoration is about actually copying in some cases literal civil codes that were copied from Europe that have all these rules about marriage and the family. And in societies with high levels of conformity, hierarchical decision making, you can implement these things much more quickly than amongst a bunch of disagreeable individualists.
So what would be great is if I could predict the future, right? But, at least based on that, the kind of East Asian part fits. Now we’ve tried to deal with your other question in the following way. We have nighttime satellite illumination data from the entire world. And we think we can use that as a proxy for economic growth. And then we have the map which appears in the book of the intensity of different kinship structures around the world.
And what we can do is we can look at places like Latin America and Africa and elsewhere, and we just compare different ethnic groups in the same country. So we’re removing all the country level stuff, country level politics from this and just comparing ethnic groups within the same country. And what we find is that ethnic groups that had more intensive kinship traditionally are darker, averaging data from right around 2010 or so. So it looks like economic prosperity measured as light illumination is associated with groups that traditionally had weaker kinship intensity.
And our story is that because the institutions built in Europe are the ones that have expanded around the world and now dominate, they fit within the kind of psychology and the social structure that goes along with low-intensity kinship structure. And so it’s easier for them to engage in world markets and become more prosperous.
- ezra klein
One thing I find interesting about the point you just made on some of the East Asian societies is that it does go in the opposite direction of one of the concerns I was raising earlier, which is what you’re saying is that they were able to impose, much more directly and much more by force, what the Catholic Church did indirectly, right? If the Catholic Church were saying, well, God doesn’t want you to do this, China just said, well, we don’t want you to do this. And, actually, we’re going to put a much sharper constraint around it than anyone else will.
And I’m not here to support the one child policy. I do not support the one child policy, but it does, I think, suggest — and not, again, taking the point that China went way, way, way too far on this and had very bad years as part of that, it does in a way suggest that there is the ability to run this kind of social cooperation and limiting game more directly.
- joseph henrich
Yeah. Although, the interesting part — I guess, China is kind of a test case moving forward because it’s easiest for China to do that earlier on the more hierarchical, the more conformist people are. As that process begins to move along and people are becoming more individualistic and whatnot, the ability of the state to do that, to work through elders, to rely on conformity and stuff, will get trickier. Although, I guess technology could compensate.
- ezra klein
I actually had wanted to ask you about technology because you’ve talked a lot here about the ways in which cultural evolution can be set off and can run indirectly through religion and other kinds of things that create a lot of human cooperation. But technology also seems to me to be a force that certain technologies will emerge. The birth control pill is a very canonical example here, but you can also think about the factory or air conditioning.
And all kinds of new forms of cooperation, new settlement patterns, new family structures, new ways of structuring out the life cycle begin to emerge from that. So how do you think about technology and technological change within the context of cultural evolution?
- joseph henrich
The first point is I agree that for sure technology can have those kinds of effects. I think about technology as itself a product of cultural evolution. The way social science kind of works, sometimes, people want to put technology in its own bin separate from culture, but it’s very clear that people learn. If you just go to the smallest scale of human societies, people learn how to make the relevant tools from other members of their community. This is passed on.
If you look at the larger scale societies, people still have to learn how to do whatever engineering stuff in the factories, making the tools from other people. This could be passed along indirectly through books and things like that. But there’s lots of tacit knowledge that goes into even modern industrial production that has to be moved from one craftsperson or one engineer to the next. So it’s all cultural transmission all the way down.
And one of the things I focus on the book is, what are the conditions that lead to more rapid technological change? So I have this idea of the collective brain, which is that the particular institutions in a certain place and the psychology, trust in strangers, things like that, lack of conformity will actually energize and create more rapid innovation.
So at the end of “The Weirdest People in the World,” I’m trying to explain the Industrial Revolution, and why did you have this explosion of new technologies, which did change things in the way you mentioned, all at once beginning in the English Midlands and then spreading out from there?
- ezra klein
And talk me through a little bit more your explanation for that.
- joseph henrich
Yeah. So my story is that most ideas are recombinations of existing ideas already circulating in the cultural milieu. So the more diverse minds are able to interact and share ideas, the more likely you are to create novel recombinations and lead to the diffusion of ideas. Even the famous geniuses like Einstein, if you look at special relativity, it’s a combination of a set of ideas that others were in the process of putting together, and he just happened to be the first guy to really congeal them together.
But this is true of lots of different technologies. In a book I’m writing now, we actually have data from the complete U.S Patent Database showing that nearly all patents are actually combinations of existing technologies. So that seems to be the case. And then larger cities, as a piece of evidence, tend to produce more patents per capita. So more creativity per capita. And that’s just the idea that there are more people closer together, and it kind of makes everyone smarter. You’re more likely to bump into someone who has something useful that you put together with something else, and then you’re off and running.
Things like the spread of Starbucks seems to energize patenting, which is a surprising thing to a lot of people because people had a third place where people from different companies could come together, swap ideas. And you can actually look at the details of what the new patents are and see that happening.
- ezra klein
One thing that recurs throughout some of the stories of the book is that a lot of these religious and cultural changes made possible more migration. And more migration made possible more mixing of ideas, more innovation, a lot of what we take for granted now in modernity. How do you think about migration in the modern world? Is that something that if you are kind of architecting societies for more innovation and more cultural and more rapid cultural evolution that you would be trying to turn up the dial on?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, and I have a big project on this. So we’ve combined the full complete U.S census with the complete Patent Database. And what we’re able to show is that migrant shocks to the U.S., we can look at what counties they end up in. And then we can measure the diversity of those counties by looking at last names as they appear in the census. And what we find is that what we’re calling sociocultural diversity, which is this measure of last name variation, is a huge predictor of patents in the coming decade.
And immigrant shocks add richness to that diversity. So it’s just more evidence that immigrants have really driven a lot of innovation in the U.S. And we wondered. We said, well, maybe immigrants, if you have too many, it will cause problems. It might cause problems, but we can’t find any kink in the curve. This seems to be more immigrants, more innovation.
So we looked for a hum shape. You might say, maybe there’s an optimal number of immigrants, but it doesn’t look like the U.S. has hit that. And there’s very interesting papers done by Petra Moser and colleagues showing that when the U.S. put in immigration quotas in 1924 cutting out or limiting from certain populations, you got a decline in the group and the patenting domains that those groups had previously been attributing to. And it even meant that Americans got less creative in those domains. So the immigrants are not just bringing in ideas that make themselves rich; they’re bringing in ideas that they’re mixing with the native-borns and making everybody more creative.
- ezra klein
Speaking of county-by-county data, I know something you’ve been working on is mapping, within America, how psychologically WEIRD, again, on this one scale a county is against how it seems to be voting. And I know that research isn’t out yet, but is there anything preliminarily you are seeing in the data that’s interesting?
- joseph henrich
Yeah. So this begins with a paper by my colleague Ben Enke in economics at Harvard. And what Ben did was he went to this — there’s a psychologist named Jon Haidt, who’s been collecting data at yourmorals.org, which you can go in there, you answer a bunch of questions, and it kind of puts you on a place within a five-dimensional morality scheme.
And those dimensions are cool. But to simplify things, you can simplify this down to one big and most important dimension which runs from moral universalism where people are seeing others as more or less equal. The in group, out group difference is minimized. People care about a lot of harm and fairness, not so much about hierarchy and loyalty and some of these other dimensions of morality. Down to the more parochial end, where people have a smaller circle of cooperation and they’re very concerned about loyalties and hierarchy.
So you get this spectrum. And you can measure that at the U.S. county level because the data set is rich enough. And what Ben is able to show — and this is published in a top econ journal. It’s a very reliable result. A bunch of back up studies and stuff — is that counties that were more morally parochial were more likely to vote for Donald Trump. And it was that vote total over and above what Mitt Romney or John McCain got. So it’s the sort of extra Trump boost from being high on moral parochialism.
Well, we wanted to know if that would replicate for 2020, and that seemed to be the case there. And then we tried to explain the variation. So there’s this county-level variation. And the first thing to note is that if you plot from 2008 to 2019, what you see is a divergence. So the urban areas maintain, maybe they go up a little bit in moral universalism, whereas the rural counties are going down the whole time, declining in their moral universalism, getting more parochial, OK? So we have temporal variation.
And then the question is, can we explain that?
And the two factors that came out of our first set of analyzes is, first, shocks of any kind. So economic shocks seem to make people more morally parochial, and weather shocks. So climate change hit some U.S counties more than other U.S. counties. And if you get hit with a shock, hurricane, say, flood, fire, you get a bit more morally parochial.
And the final thing that seemed to matter — and we’re not sure how to deal with this — is that movement. Americans have been moving less and less, and they’re staying put more. But places that stayed disproportionately more, meaning most people stayed where they were born, seem to also be more morally parochial.
And that makes sense with this kind of distant ties idea that you move to a new place and you’ve got to make new friends and you create new social networks. If you stay in the same place, you kind of establish long-term family and friend ties and whatnot. And that works towards more morally parochialism. So that could be related to the economic shocks and stuff, and the sort of directions of the causal hours there that’s causing us some challenges.
- ezra klein
One obvious trend we seem to be seeing over the past 100 years, past 50, I think past 20 is the world is getting weirder, right? This directionality seems to be happening in a lot of places. Does that become a worrying monoculture at some point? It was a dimension of cultural evolution. But if it becomes everywhere, do you get a situation where there’s not enough diversity, where we’re, as a species, overindexed on individualism or something like that?
Do you worry about the world getting too weird? Is it plausible the competitive advantage will be going in the other direction, sort of like the Mormons in America? Do you see something happening here where what’s been a competitive advantage will become a disadvantage, or at least something that is more widespread than would probably be optimal?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, this is a very common question. And what’s behind the question is that there’s this linear trajectory that we’ve been going on. And I guess I think that what I’m seeing — and I don’t have any quantitative data to offer you. But as what were European institutions, take universities or Democratic government, has spread to all these different places. In some cases, it’s a copy of the U.S. Constitution is spread widely. But then it has to interface with the existing local institutions.
So if you just take we were talking about China and Japan before. I think what’s emerging there is somewhat weirder psychology, but the societies are often also going off in new directions. So think of something more complicated than a two-dimensional line we’re going along. So a one-dimensional line, but rather there’s different ways of going. So I don’t think there’s really a kind of unilineal thing going on here. I think or something more complicated going on.
And what we need to do is just do the more psychological work to really map the space of all the different possibilities and see where things are going. To what degree — in China, there are some capitalist-type institutions there are some free markets. But, obviously, at the political level, there’s not democracy. But, although, at the lower level, there is democracy. So it’s complicated and we haven’t sorted it all out yet.
- ezra klein
Well, when you look on psychological surveys, how are people differing? When you say something new is emerging, are we able to see on the same surveys where people are showing a different set of preferences or tendencies?
- joseph henrich
Yeah, so one example that makes China challenging — and this kind of illustrates the bigger problem — is that if you ask people to generalize trust question, which is the survey, they seem very trusting. But then if you ask the more detailed set of questions — do you trust foreigners, do you trust people you met for the first time versus people who live around you — and you analyze those, they don’t look very trusting.
So in Australia and the U.S., if you ask people the generalized trust question, it goes along with, it’s correlated with that in group, out group trust I was describing. Whereas in China, they seem to be unrelated. So it’s as if life is structuring itself in a way so that most people you interact with, you trust. But if you ask me about foreigners, I don’t trust those people. So that’s the more complicated picture. And I haven’t seen any analyses yet that look for the kind of multidimensional shape that I’m talking about.
- ezra klein
I think that is a good place to come to an end. So always our final question, what are the three books you would recommend to the audience?
- joseph henrich
Well, let’s see. I really like “Why Europe?” by Michael Mitterauer if you wanted to learn more about Europe. I still think everybody should read Jared Diamond’s “Guns, Germs, and Steel.” And I really like this book, “The Chosen Few: How Education Shaped Jewish History,” because it really emphasizes how early changes in a religion of a particular group had big, long-term consequences.
- ezra klein
Joe Henrich, thank you very much.
- joseph henrich
Great to be with you. [MUSIC PLAYING]
This episode of “The Ezra Klein Show” was produced by Rogé Karma. Fact checking by Michelle Harris, Mary Marge Locker, and Kate Sinclair. Mixing by Sonia Herrero. Our production team is Emefa Agawu, Annie Galvin, Jeff Geld, Rogé Karma, and Kristin Lin. Original music by Isaac Jones. Audience strategy by Shannon Busta. The executive producer of New York Times Opinion Audio is Annie-Rose Strasser. And special thanks to Kristina Samulewski. [MUSIC PLAYING]
Here’s a little experiment. Take a second to think about how you would fill in the blank in this sentence: “I am _____.”
If you’re anything like me, the first descriptors that come to mind are personal attributes (like “curious” or “kind”) or identities (like “a journalist” or “a runner”). And if you answered that way, then I have some news for you: You are weird.
I mean that in a very specific way. In social science, WEIRD is an acronym that stands for “Western, educated, industrialized, rich and democratic.” Most societies in the world today — and throughout human history — don’t fit that description. And when people from non-WEIRD cultures answer the “I am” statement, they tend to give very different answers, defining themselves with relation-based descriptors like “Moe’s father” or “David’s brother.”
That difference is only the tip of the iceberg. Much of what we take for granted as basic elements of human psychology and ethics are actually a peculiarly WEIRD way of viewing the world.
[You can listen to this episode of “The Ezra Klein Show” on Apple, Spotify, Amazon Music, Google or wherever you get your podcasts.]
Joseph Henrich, an anthropologist at Harvard University, believes that this distinction between WEIRD and non-WEIRD psychologies is absolutely central to understanding our modern world. His 2020 book, “The WEIRDest People in the World: How the West Became Psychologically Peculiar and Particularly Prosperous,” explores the origins of these differences and argues that the emergence of a distinctly WEIRD psychology was central to the development of everything from the Industrial Revolution and market economies to representative government and human rights.
We discuss Henrich’s theory of how “cultural evolution” leads to psychological — even genetic — changes in humans, the difference between societies that experience shame as a dominant emotion as opposed to guilt, the unique power of religion in driving cultural change, how cultural inventions like reading have literally reshaped human biology, why religious communes tend to outlast secular ones, why Henrich believes there is no static human nature aside from our cultural learning abilities, how differences in moral psychology across the United States could predict Donald Trump’s 2016 and 2020 vote share, why higher levels of immigration tend to lead to far more innovation and more.
You can listen to our whole conversation by following “The Ezra Klein Show” on Apple, Spotify, Google or wherever you get your podcasts. View a list of book recommendations from our guests here.
(A full transcript of the episode is available here.)

This episode of “The Ezra Klein Show” was produced by Roge Karma. Fact-checking by Michelle Harris, Mary Marge Locker and Kate Sinclair. Mixing by Sonia Herrero. Our production team is Emefa Agawu, Annie Galvin, Jeff Geld and Kristin Lin. Original music by Isaac Jones. Audience strategy by Shannon Busta. The executive producer of New York Times Opinion Audio is Annie-Rose Strasser. Special thanks to Kristina Samulewski.
Advertisement